Animal Brain size, mass, and surface area

In summary: There is no definitive answer.In summary, according to this study, the allometric scaling component for ants (b = 0.57) was similar to that for birds (b = 0.58) and reptiles (b = 0.54) but significantly different from that of mammals (b = 0.77).
  • #1
Martin_G
21
0
Animal Brain size, body mass, and surface area

S.J. Gould wrote a series of interesting articles (chapters 22-24 in Ever Since Darwin) about the ratio of body mass to brain mass among animal species. Bigger animals have bigger brains (of course) but the ratio is getting smaller: a human's brain is smaller than an ant's relative to body size. Gould conjectures that this is due to volume to surface area ratio: since volumes (and therefore masses) increase faster than surface areas, an ant has a larger surface area to body mass ratio. Therefore, the ant needs a relatively bigger brain to control and sense its body surface area, with its appendages, nerves, etc.

Question: this explanation sounds reasonable, but Gould provides no evidence. Does anyone know of evidence to support that hypothesis, or of other hypotheses regarding that phenomenon?
 
Last edited:
Biology news on Phys.org
  • #3
Thanks, I looked it up and I also read one of the reviews referenced there. However, that doesn't answer the question. The enchephalization quotient quantifies a species' distance from the log-log regression line; it doesn't explain why the line has the slope it does (2/3), and it doesn't even explain why the slope is less than 1.
 
  • #4
I don't really know why surface area would have that much of an influence on brain size. I guess I'd look at it more as a limit issue. When you're going from a teeny tiny ganglion to an actual brain and increasing in complexity, initially, I'd expect an increase in brain size with organism complexity, but I'd expect that to begin to plateau as function stabilizes although size increases. For example, if a cat and human have the same basic analogous muscles, while the neurons in a human may need more motor endplate branches than in a cat to control the larger muscles, there's no compelling reason why you'd need more neurons in the brain for that purpose. There are differences in size, but they become less proportional.

Steven Gould is primarily an evolutionary biologist, not a neuroscientist, so when he begins to conjecture outside of his field, consider it just that, conjecture.
 
  • #5
Moonbear,

1. I think your reasoning is similar to Gould's: a bigger (that is, heavier) body without much more sophistication would require a proportionally smaller brain. Gould made a similar argument going the other way around: the ant's smaller (lighter) body has larger relative surface area which requires a relatively bigger brain.

2. This is not a limit issue. It's true for all animals from ants to blue whales, and doesn't level off at any point.

3. Gould was not even an evolutionary biologist, as his detractors often mention. He was a paleontologist. The chapters I mentioned are so badly written that one wonders whether they were written by a child (he describes all animals as geometric cubes). Nevertheless, some of the most interesting research and conjectures come from those who step outside of their fields, e.g. Roger Penrose, Len Adleman, Schrödinger, etc.
 
  • #6
Martin_G said:
1. I think your reasoning is similar to Gould's: a bigger (that is, heavier) body without much more sophistication would require a proportionally smaller brain. Gould made a similar argument going the other way around: the ant's smaller (lighter) body has larger relative surface area which requires a relatively bigger brain.

The allometric scaling argument seems sound, though there is always the question of whether you can reduce the reason to some single factor like circulation efficiency in the case of metabolic rate (Kleiber's law) or neural mass to body surface area in this case. In a rough approximation at least, brain mass is going to grow by the cube while body surface grows by the square.

So Gould was really applying a biological null hypothesis to the brain - it is standard thinking, the obvious place to start.

But you were asking about slope. If it is just a surface/mass relationship, then slope would be 2/3.

The slope is in fact different across the animal phyla. From http://si-pddr.si.edu/jspui/bitstre...llometry_of_Brain_Miniaturization_in_Ants.pdf

That study demonstrated that the allometric scaling component for ants (b = 0.57) was similar to that for birds (b = 0.58) and reptiles (b = 0.54) but significantly different from that of mammals (b = 0.77).

The interesting finding of this study is that the relationship breaks down with extreme miniaturisation in the case of ants.

To our knowledge, extremely small ants ( ! 0.9 mg body mass) are the only animals known to have a brain allometric coefficient comparable to that of mammals [White et al., 2009].

But I think the honest answer with brain allometry studies is that there is a lot of speculation.

For example, some say dolphins are "above the line" simply because they are pygmy whales rather than because they have evolved larger brains due to social intelligence or echolocation. One or other might be the dominant factor. It is quite easy to fit the same data to different explanations.

However at the phyla level, the basic observation that brains follow some kind of scaling law goes right back to Haller in the 1700s. And Gould's reasoning would be the plausible starting point for most.
 
  • #7
In a rough approximation at least, brain mass is going to grow by the cube while body surface grows by the square.

That would only be true for a cube, and I think that was one of Gould's errors. Consider two cylinders of the same radius r, one of height h and the other of radius 2h. The second has twice the volume of the first, but what can you say about the ratio of their surface areas? It could be almost identical, if the height is far smaller than the radius (pancake), or it could approach 2 if height is far greater than radius (spaghetti). Unless I'm missing something, it's basic solid geometry. If I were to choose a simple geometric shape that approximates a living organism, I'd choose a cylinder or a rectangular cuboid other than a cube.
 
  • #8
Martin_G said:
That would only be true for a cube, and I think that was one of Gould's errors. Consider two cylinders of the same radius r, one of height h and the other of radius 2h. The second has twice the volume of the first, but what can you say about the ratio of their surface areas? It could be almost identical, if the height is far smaller than the radius (pancake), or it could approach 2 if height is far greater than radius (spaghetti). Unless I'm missing something, it's basic solid geometry. If I were to choose a simple geometric shape that approximates a living organism, I'd choose a cylinder or a rectangular cuboid other than a cube.

The brain grows by the cube - it does form a pretty circular lump because one of its design constraints is its need to be highly internally connected.

Whereas the surface area of the body grows by the square (of any volume increase). But yes, this is an idealisation as bodies are not actually spherical blobs. They are far more varied.

However within phyla, body plans would be similar enough so that some constant surface area relationship existed. Which would set things up for a ratio between the brain and body surface area.
 
  • #9
I'm sorry, but the more I think about this, the less it makes sense to me.

What does volumes grow by the cube mean? What does area grows by the square mean? These are, I think, utterly meaningless statements from a mathematical and physical standpoint, except for the obvious: volume is three-dimensional, area is two-dimensional. I was wondering about this when I read Gould, but I was more interested in his biological insights, which are fascinating, than in his deeply flawed math (and politics, but we'll leave that alone).

These statements only make sense if we say that the ratio of volume to surface area of a cube of side length x is x/6. Therefore, the ratio grows linearly with x. Outside that context, I see no sense in these statements.

As for the human body, it's pretty far from a cube, and any approximation that treats it as such would perforce be way off. As a rough approximation, I'd say that the human body approximates a rectangular cuboid of dimensions roughly in a ratio of 9:2:1. For a dog, I'd go with 5:4:1.

Then, we have to decide what growth occurs in each of the three dimensions.

So, I retract all references to Gould and to surface area in my original post, but the original question remains: why do bigger bodies require proportional smaller brains?
Moonbear made a reasonable hypothesis, but I wonder whether there are more.
 
  • #10
Martin_G said:
What does volumes grow by the cube mean? What does area grows by the square mean? .

Does this help?

cem2s1_1.jpg
 
  • #11
About as much as this does:
depositphotos_7225773-Two-plus-two-equals-four.jpg


I stated that the cube case is trivial four times already.
Can you rigorously state volume grows by the cube for an arbitrary geometric shape?
 
  • #12
Martin_G said:
These statements only make sense if we say that the ratio of volume to surface area of a cube of side length x is x/6. Therefore, the ratio grows linearly with x. Outside that context, I see no sense in these statements.

As for the human body, it's pretty far from a cube, and any approximation that treats it as such would perforce be way off. As a rough approximation, I'd say that the human body approximates a rectangular cuboid of dimensions roughly in a ratio of 9:2:1. For a dog, I'd go with 5:4:1.

Then, we have to decide what growth occurs in each of the three dimensions.
In the general case, a general creature is going to scale up proportionally. It does not matter about the shape. The same could be said about an amoeba.

Double its height and its length and breadth will double as well, preserving all dimensions.

At least, ideally.

If a man were scaled up to twice his height, so he's now 12ft tall, his weight won't double, it will octuple.

Unfortunately, this won't happen in real life because of the square cube law (Google this). The mans' legs have a cross-sectional area, and in doubling all dimensions, his corss section has only squared not octupled. His legs are now supporting twice as much weight per unit area.

Let's say a 6 ft man has the following dimensions:
6ft tall, 2 ft across, 1 foot through, his mass is 100kg, and his leg cross section is 100cm2 (10cmx10cm)

His legs are supporting 1kg per cm2

Scale this man up to 12 ft and he is
12ft tall, 4 ft across, 2 foot through, his mass is 800kg, and his leg cross section is 400cm2(20cmx20cm)

His legs are supporting 2kg per cm2.

This double-sized man will collapse under his own weight.
 
  • #13
My above post was a little premature. You're not convinced about the linear-cube relationship.
Martin_G said:
Can you rigorously state volume grows by the cube for an arbitrary geometric shape?
It does. Feel l free to pick a shape at whim and try it to prove it to yourself.
A sphere octuples in volume when its radius doubles.
Note that you can make any arbitrary shape built from a collection of small spheres. So, doubling the linear measurement of any shape will octuple its volume and mass.Look, uh, if you're just learning this, perhaps you could be a little less snarky and make fewer assertions about what is trivial and what is fundamental.
 
Last edited:
  • #14
yes, Martin. This i why mechanical engineers know that giant insects are not possible as they are now structured. Compressing force of gravity goes with volume (cubed) while structural integrity goes with surface area (squared). So if you keep making an insect bigger and bigger, it gets to the point where it's weight can't be supported by the surface area.
 
  • #15
Pythagorean said:
it's weight can't be supported by the surface area.
Cross-sectional area.
 
  • #16
Martin_G said:
I stated that the cube case is trivial four times already.
Can you rigorously state volume grows by the cube for an arbitrary geometric shape?

You just seem to want a pointless argument.

I already said...

However within phyla, body plans would be similar enough so that some constant surface area relationship existed. Which would set things up for a ratio between the brain and body surface area.
 
  • #17
DaveC426913 said:
Cross-sectional area.

oops, thanks.
 
  • #18
Dave,

Don't patronize me. I'm not learning anything here, as nothing said in the last few posts is more than trivial and obvious. I get that 23=8. You spent a lot of words and numbers to demonstrate that, but I got it in the third grade or so.

You, on the other hand, seem to assume that doubling one dimension will necessarily be accompanied by doubling of the two other dimensions, and that this holds across species. That may be true, but I see no reason to take it as obvious, and stating 23=8 for the eleventh time using nice pictures will not help to convince me.

Besides, none of that has anything to do with my question, anyway.

apeiron,
However within phyla, body plans would be similar enough so that some constant surface area relationship existed

Indeed, the stingray and the elephant have very similar body plans...
 
  • #19
Martin_G said:
I'm not learning anything here...
Certainly not. You sure have a novel way of going about, getting irritable, demanding and insulting.

Why are you annoyed with us exactly? Who do you think is responsible for your failing to grasp the principles we're sharing with you? Ours?
Martin_G said:
Don't patronize me. I'm not learning anything here, as nothing said in the last few posts is more than trivial and obvious. I get that 23=8. You spent a lot of words and numbers to demonstrate that, but I got it in the third grade or so.
It was you who asked:

What does volumes grow by the cube mean? What does area grows by the square mean?
Don't get snarky because we provided you with the answer to a question you asked.
Martin_G said:
You, on the other hand, seem to assume that doubling one dimension will necessarily be accompanied by doubling of the two other dimensions, and that this holds across species. That may be true, but I see no reason to take it as obvious, and stating 23=8 for the eleventh time using nice pictures will not help to convince me.
In the general case, as I said - it is true. Once we have the general formula, we can consider what might change about a given animal.

You had not grasped the general case. The principle involved.
 
Last edited:
  • #20
As much as I enjoy that intellectual exchange, the Snooker Masters is on, so that will be my last post here. The "general case", the "principle involved" of which you boast, is the third-grade statement that a three-dimensional object has three dimensions (gasp!), and thus doubling all three will lead to an eightfold increase in volume. I guess a small mind will capture that fact with the slogan "volume increases by the cube", but some of us aren't satisfied by slogans, as my cylinder examples show. Now go apply that brilliant slogan to a cobra, a stingray, and an elephant (same body plan, according to apeiron), and tell me how it worked out. I'm done arguing with you.
 
  • #21
Martin_G said:
Indeed, the stingray and the elephant have very similar body plans...

Which is why Haller's rule is based on the ratio of brain weight to body weight.

But the question was about Gould's speculation about the reason for this observed relationship - whether it is connected to the weight of neurons needed to control some amount of body surface. The figures look about right as a rough approximation.

Gould was I think extrapolating in rather direct fashion from the well-known Kleiber's law which relates metabolic rate and body surface area. See - http://universe-review.ca/R10-35-metabolic.htm

So that seems a reasonable starting point. And its seems likely that a neural volume to area-to-control relation exists. But just as Kleiber's law now stresses the fractal nature of dissipative biological organisation, so brain allometry probably also is more about neural volume to a fractal internal "area-to-control". Which would change the expected slope of the scaling relationship.

However, there is a lot else going on that would confound any kind of simple relationship here.

Social behaviour demands larger brains usually. So this would produce correlations between brain size and lifestyle rather than body plan. Except there are reports from ant studies that argue small ants instead make do with smaller more specialised brains as the social behaviour is emergent and does not have to be held as a whole in each little head.

Another thing going on is the increasing complexity of internal control. So there is a switch between cold blooded and warm blooded that changes the slope of the relationship. Which again is not about surface area to control issues.

But then birds are another story because flight creates strong weight constraints and so they have brains which have "encephalised" by a different route - in simple terms, the striatum is what has grown rather than the cortex.

Yet another speculation is that there has been an evolutionary intelligence arms race so you will see larger brains in more modern branches of the family tree. The "marsupials and insectivores are dumber" line of argument.

(Much of this is dredged up from dim memory from a very comprehensive review on the subject - Brain and Mind, edited by David Oakley - which sits in a box in my garage somewhere :smile:).

Then there are much more recent confounding studies that point both ways on how to think about the issues.

One is the finding that baby spiders have brains that extend into their legs as if neural mass is so critical that it must invade other areas until the body grows. But also a finding that miniature parasitic wasps are nearly isometrically scaled - instead of small species having relatively large brains, the brains shrink in stricter proportion. This could involve a shrinking of the neurons themselves, or a shrinkage in performance - less capacity for memory, etc.

So the point is that Kleiber's law does cover the whole animal kingdom and the causal explanation seems pretty straight forward. It is a simple fractal dissipative relationship. And it explains things like why elephants need big ears. Or why cats curl up in a ball if they are cold.

But when it comes to brain allometry, even Haller's rule does not hold that well across the animal kingdom. The miniature insect brain has become an interesting area of research now because of the contradictions appearing.

And it seems unlikely that one single factor will account for the relationships. However, the encephalisation quotient does draw a straight line average which gives a phyla-level benchmark that may pick out a basic brain mass to body mass "generalised control" relationship. And then animals with more social lifestyles lie above the baseline. And those from more primitive clades lie below.
 
  • #22
Martin_G said:
As much as I enjoy that intellectual exchange, the Snooker Masters is on, so that will be my last post here. The "general case", the "principle involved" of which you boast, is the third-grade statement that a three-dimensional object has three dimensions (gasp!), and thus doubling all three will lead to an eightfold increase in volume. I guess a small mind will capture that fact with the slogan "volume increases by the cube", but some of us aren't satisfied by slogans, as my cylinder examples show. Now go apply that brilliant slogan to a cobra, a stingray, and an elephant (same body plan, according to apeiron), and tell me how it worked out. I'm done arguing with you.

Seriously Martin, this is a very strange reaction to people trying to answer your questions. OK, we may not be on the same wavelength with you, but really, you're being kind of a jerk about it.
 

What is the average brain size of animals?

The average brain size of animals varies greatly depending on the species. For example, the average brain size of a human is around 1300-1400 cubic centimeters, while the average brain size of an elephant is around 4000-5000 cubic centimeters.

Does brain size determine intelligence in animals?

There is no direct correlation between brain size and intelligence in animals. While larger brains may be associated with higher cognitive abilities, there are many other factors that contribute to intelligence, such as social behavior, problem-solving skills, and adaptability.

Which animals have the largest brains?

The animal with the largest brain is the sperm whale, which can have a brain mass of up to 9 kilograms. Other animals with large brains include elephants, dolphins, and humans.

Is there a relationship between brain size and body size in animals?

There is a general trend of larger animals having larger brains, but there are exceptions to this rule. For example, the brain-to-body mass ratio of a mouse is higher than that of an elephant, meaning the mouse's brain is proportionally larger compared to its body size.

What is the role of surface area in brain size?

The surface area of the brain plays a crucial role in its function. A larger surface area allows for more connections and interactions between neurons, leading to more complex cognitive abilities. However, this does not necessarily mean that animals with larger brains have a larger surface area, as the structure and folding of the brain also play a significant role in its function.

Similar threads

  • Biology and Medical
Replies
1
Views
840
  • Biology and Medical
Replies
2
Views
1K
  • General Discussion
Replies
18
Views
2K
  • Sci-Fi Writing and World Building
Replies
21
Views
1K
  • Science Fiction and Fantasy Media
Replies
0
Views
971
  • General Discussion
Replies
7
Views
4K
  • Precalculus Mathematics Homework Help
Replies
4
Views
11K
  • Introductory Physics Homework Help
Replies
7
Views
2K
  • Introductory Physics Homework Help
Replies
14
Views
3K
  • Electromagnetism
Replies
28
Views
2K
Back
Top