Bayes, Hume, lotteries and trustworty newspapers

  • I
  • Thread starter haushofer
  • Start date
In summary, the conversation discusses the probability of actually winning a lottery after being reported as the winner by a newspaper. It is argued that although the probability of winning is extremely small, the probability of the newspaper reporting the correct outcome is much bigger. This is compared to a medical test for a rare disease, where Bayes' theorem is used to calculate the probability of actually being infected after a positive test result. It is also questioned whether this argument can be applied to the lottery case, as the newspaper does not influence the outcome of the lottery. The conversation concludes with a discussion on the probability of the newspaper's report being correct and how it can be estimated.
  • #36
stevendaryl said:
There is a slight variant of the lottery example that makes a huge difference in the computed probability. Instead of the newspaper reporting the lottery winner with 98% accuracy, assume that there is a website where you can enter your ticket number, and it tells you, with 98% accuracy, whether you are the winner or not. Even though that sounds similar to the original problem, in this case, it's much more likely that the website is in error than that you are the actual winner.
I have to think about that one.
 
Physics news on Phys.org
  • #37
I think I worked out the lottery now, making some extra assumptions about the report. Maybe I'll put it here for completeness.

stevendaryl said:
There is a slight variant of the lottery example that makes a huge difference in the computed probability. Instead of the newspaper reporting the lottery winner with 98% accuracy, assume that there is a website where you can enter your ticket number, and it tells you, with 98% accuracy, whether you are the winner or not. Even though that sounds similar to the original problem, in this case, it's much more likely that the website is in error than that you are the actual winner.
Could you explain this in more detail?
 
  • #38
haushofer said:
I think I worked out the lottery now, making some extra assumptions about the report. Maybe I'll put it here for completeness.Could you explain this in more detail?

Imagine the following process:
  1. You call the lottery office.
  2. They spin a dial to get a real number between 0 and 1.
  3. If the number is less than 0.98, they tell you the truth about whether you won the lottery or not.
  4. If the number is between 0.98 and 1, they lie to you about it.
Now, you call up the office and ask whether you won the lottery, or not. Before you even talk to anyone, there are 4 possibilities:
  1. You won the lottery and they tell you the truth. The probability of this is ##10^{-10} \cdot 0.98##
  2. You won the lottery and they lie to you: The probability of this is ##0^{-10} \cdot 0.02##
  3. You did not win the lottery, and they tell you the truth. The probability of this is: ##(1-10^{-10}) \cdot 0.98##
  4. You did not win the lottery, and they tell you a lie. The probability of this is: ##(1-10^{-10}) \cdot 0.02##
After you talk to someone and they tell you that you won, you can eliminate possibilities 2 and 3. So that leaves 1 and 4. 4 is much more likely than 1.
 
Last edited:
  • Like
Likes haushofer
  • #39
The newspaper equivalent would be "if you don't win and they print a wrong name, they always print your name".
 
  • Like
Likes stevendaryl
  • #40
stevendaryl said:
Imagine the following process:
  1. You call the lottery office.
  2. They spin a dial to get a real number between 0 and 1.
  3. If the number is less than 0.98, they tell you the truth about whether you won the lottery or not.
  4. If the number is between 0.98 and 1, they lie to you about it.
Now, you call up the office and ask whether you won the lottery, or not. Before you even talk to anyone, there are 4 possibilities:
  1. You won the lottery and they tell you the truth. The probability of this is ##10^{-10} \cdot 0.98##
  2. You won the lottery and they lie to you: The probability of this is ##0^{-10} \cdot 0.02##
  3. You did not win the lottery, and they tell you the truth. The probability of this is: ##(1-10^{-10}) \cdot 0.98##
  4. You did not win the lottery, and they tell you a lie. The probability of this is: ##(1-10^{-10}) \cdot 0.02##
After you talk to someone and they tell you that you won, you can eliminate possibilities 2 and 3. So that leaves 1 and 4. 4 is much more likely than 1.
Thanks, that makes sense. (you missed a 1 in option 2, btw; ##0^{-10} \rightarrow 10^{-10}## ;) )

I must say I like this lotterystuff; it shows the subtleties involved in these calculations.
 
  • #41
So let me write down how I worked out this example. I assumed three things:

* (1) the lottery is fair, so a priori every number has the same probability of winning
* (2) every winning lottery number has the same probability of being printed
* (3) every losing lottery number has the same probability of being printed

So there are no biases concerning rightly or wrongly printing. Now imagine the lottery numbers range from 1 to 10.000.000. We (as PF-forum, happy to share) drew number 42. And indeed, the newspaper reports that number 42 won!

The a priori probability of lottery number ##x## to win is denoted as ##P(x)##, and the data that the newspaper reports that number ##y## won is denoted as ##P(paper:y)##. So we have, according to (1), ##P(42)=\frac{1}{10000000}=10^{-7}##. Also, a reliability of 98% means that ##P(paper:42 | 42)=0.98##. And finally, from (3), we can deduce that ##P(paper:42 | \neg 42) = \frac{0.02}{9999999}##; all of the remaining 9.999.999 (all of the numbers besides 42) have the same probability of being printed wrongly. If I put these numbers into Bayes, I get

[tex]
P(42|\text{paper:}42) = \frac{P(\text{paper:}42|42) P(42)}{P(\text{paper:}42|42) P(42) + P(\text{paper:}42|\neg 42)P(\neg 42)}
[/tex]

which gives me as answer

[tex]
P(42|\text{paper:}42) = 0,9998 = 99.98 \%
[/tex]

The only thing I'm not sure of whether it makes sense that this number exceeds the initial reliability of 98%. But this should be right, right?

-edit: resolved, made an arithmetic error in converting percentages.
 
Last edited:
  • #42
mfb said:
The newspaper equivalent would be "if you don't win and they print a wrong name, they always print your name".

Yes. So I guess that's why

##P(paper:42 | \neg 42) = \frac{0.02}{9999999}##

instead of

##P(paper:42 | \neg 42) = 0.02 ##.
 
  • #43
Assume you only have one ticket and there is only one winner/number published...

If your number is in the paper your odds of having won = 49/50.

But its also possible you won and the paper made a mistake = 1/10m x 1/50

Just add these together.
 
  • #44
haushofer said:
So let me write down how I worked out this example. I assumed three things:

* (1) the lottery is fair, so a priori every number has the same probability of winning
* (2) every winning lottery number has the same probability of being printed
* (3) every losing lottery number has the same probability of being printed

So there are no biases concerning rightly or wrongly printing. Now imagine the lottery numbers range from 1 to 10.000.000. We (as PF-forum, happy to share) drew number 42. And indeed, the newspaper reports that number 42 won!

The a priori probability of lottery number ##x## to win is denoted as ##P(x)##, and the data that the newspaper reports that number ##y## won is denoted as ##P(paper:y)##. So we have, according to (1), ##P(42)=\frac{1}{10000000}=10^{-7}##. Also, a reliability of 98% means that ##P(paper:42 | 42)=0.98##. And finally, from (3), we can deduce that ##P(paper:42 | \neg 42) = \frac{0.02}{9999999}##; all of the remaining 9.999.999 (all of the numbers besides 42) have the same probability of being printed wrongly. If I put these numbers into Bayes, I get

[tex]
P(42|\text{paper:}42) = \frac{P(\text{paper:}42|42) P(42)}{P(\text{paper:}42|42) P(42) + P(\text{paper:}42|\neg 42)P(\neg 42)}
[/tex]

which gives me as answer

[tex]
P(42|\text{paper:}42) = 0,9998 = 99.98 \%
[/tex]

The only thing I'm not sure of whether it makes sense that this number exceeds the initial reliability of 98%. But this should be right, right?

It can't be as high as that if the newspaper is only 98% accurate.
 
  • #45
PeroK said:
It can't be as high as that if the newspaper is only 98% accurate.

Mmmm, so where's the mistake then you think?
 
  • #46
haushofer said:
Mmmm, so where's the mistake then you think?
You lost me, I'm afraid. If we let A be you win the lottery and B the newspaper prints your number, then ##P(A) = P(B) = 1/N##, where N is the number of lottery tickets. This, as others have pointed out, depends on the basic assumptions of no bias.

Then, Bayes theorem reduces to:

##P(A|B) = P(B|A) = 0.98##

Another approach, which I would recommend, is to use a probability tree:
 
  • #47
... for example, if we complicate matters by the assumption that sometimes the paper prints an invalid lottery number. Say, 0.5% of the time. Then 1.5% of the time it prints a valid but incorrect winning number. And 98% of the time it prints the correct winning number.

A probability tree is ideal for handling that and avoids the difficulties of the Bayes formula as the number of options increases.
 
  • #48
PeroK said:
You lost me, I'm afraid.
haushofer said:
Mmmm, so where's the mistake then you think?
Never mind, made an arithmetic error. The probability is just 0.98 if you fill in the numbers:

[tex]
P(42|\text{paper:}42) = 0.98 = 98 \%
[/tex]

Intuitively, I understand it as the probability

##P(paper:42 | \neg 42) = \frac{0.02}{9999999}##

being too small to affect the initial reliability of ##P(paper:x | x)=0.98## of the newspaper.
 
Last edited:

Similar threads

  • Set Theory, Logic, Probability, Statistics
Replies
9
Views
393
  • Set Theory, Logic, Probability, Statistics
Replies
1
Views
2K
  • Set Theory, Logic, Probability, Statistics
Replies
19
Views
1K
  • Set Theory, Logic, Probability, Statistics
Replies
8
Views
1K
  • Set Theory, Logic, Probability, Statistics
Replies
3
Views
1K
  • Set Theory, Logic, Probability, Statistics
Replies
4
Views
3K
  • Set Theory, Logic, Probability, Statistics
Replies
7
Views
1K
  • Quantum Physics
3
Replies
87
Views
5K
  • Quantum Interpretations and Foundations
3
Replies
76
Views
4K
  • Math Proof Training and Practice
2
Replies
60
Views
8K
Back
Top