What Makes the Six Colors in Newton's Experiment Special?

  • B
  • Thread starter JB321
  • Start date
In summary: You can't have infinite shades of one color, but you can have a great many shades of different colors.
  • #36
JB321 said:
Why are there only 6 colors?
I recently read an article written by Newton which outlined the process and results of his "crucial experiment".
From what I understand, Newton says that light can continue to be split until it reaches the basic colors and then it simply stops. What is it about these frequencies of light that makes them so special? Are there six exact frequencies that are the base of all the colors? Or are there ranges of frequencies that Newton simplified to one color? I asked my physics teacher and he responded with, "I don't know..."

Thanks in advance,
JB
Being a brilliant mathematician, Newton probably figured out pretty quickly, that to name an infinite number of colors would take him an extraordinarily excessive amount of time out of his finite time here one earth, which would interfere with his further experiments.

I'm sure Newton, were he alive, would tell you; "There are more than 6 colors."
 
Physics news on Phys.org
  • #37
Simon Peach said:
So unless you're going to split a nanometre there cannot be infinite range of colours.
But of course there can be infinitely many colors in a finite range. (In fact uncountably many colors.) There is no conceptual problem with splitting a nanometer. Any two frequencies with the slightest difference represent different colors.
 
  • #38
Simon Peach said:
So unless you're going to split a nanometre there cannot be infinite range of colours.

The D-lines in the spectrum of sodium are at 588.9950 and 589.5924 nanometers. These are two instrumentally resolvable lines. Spectral lines don't just fall on integer values of nanometers.

You could say these represent two different colors. But as others have alluded to, if we mean by "color" the human visual response to light, then these are not visually distinguishable.
 
  • #39
The human visual sensors (light sensitive neurons in the retina), come in three distinct types.
These are distinguished by response to what we see as red green and blue wavelengths.
However the response is not to a precise wavelength, just are most sensitive to wavelengths in a general range.
That gives six possible combinations that our visual cortex can process, rg, rb, gr, gb, bg, br, to associate a colour to an object.
We also have sensors which don't particularly recognise wavelengths, but just total amount of light.
 
  • #40
rootone said:
The human visual sensors (light sensitive neurons in the retina), come in three distinct types.
These are distinguished by response to what we see as red green and blue wavelengths.
However the response is not to a precise wavelength, just most generally they detect wavelengths in a general range.
That gives six possible combinations that out visual cortex can process, rg, rb, gr, gb, bg, br, to associate a colour to an object.
We also have sensors which don't particularly recognise wavelengths, but just total amount of light.
Even if our cones provided only binary color recognition, two raised to the third power is eight, not six. In addition, I fail to see the distinction between gr and rg.
 
  • #41
jbriggs444 said:
Even if our cones provided only binary color recognition, two raised to the third power is eight, not six. In addition, I fail to see the distinction between gr and rg.
That is mathematically correct of course, but I'm not going to argue with nature itself, or evolution.
My personal experience is that I see six identifiable colours in a rainbow.
Not seven, I can't tell the difference between violet and indigo using that scheme
 
  • #42
rootone said:
That is mathematically correct of course, but I'm not going to argue with nature itself, or evolution.
My personal experience is that I see six identifiable colours in a rainbow.
Not seven, I can't tell the difference between violet and indigo using that scheme
That is a totally personal, subjective assessment of what you see. Under other conditions, you (despite what you claim) would almost certainly be able to distinguish between two sources of spectral colour, both of which would fall within the 'yellow' band. If what you say is true, why would they need 'millions of colours' for good quality digital colour TV?
 
  • #43
rootone said:
That gives six possible combinations that our visual cortex can process, rg, rb, gr, gb, bg, br, to associate a colour to an object.

Not sure what you are getting at here. That there are only 6 colors?
 
  • #44
Here we go:
The Four Primaries.png


I originally made this to taunt people on an artist forum who didn't have a proper understanding of color theory. But I think it's a perfectly valid approach to examining the psychological perception of colors. I honestly do see all these colors as nearly equidistant. It's hard to explain that given a trinary understanding of color. My understand is that language, and how you were taught color names as a child, can effect your ability to perceive colors. So we can blame the evil Crayola corperation for all this 6 color talk. ;)

But as for this talk about not being able to split colors further - who actually performed this experiment? Ref please? It seems to me that if you kept trying to expand a spectrum further, you would simply end up with something too dim to see the subtle changes of color.
physicswheel.png
 
  • Like
Likes rootone
  • #45
The term 'primaries' has a specific meaning. It is the least number of colours that can be mixed to produce all the others. So, with Additive Mixing (Using Lights, as in TV), you only need RG and B. There is no need for Yellow.
When mixing paints and pigments, the three primaries are Yellow, Cyan and Magenta. Other pigments are often used, to allow for brighter or more saturated colour display.
I suggest you read up about the existing theories on colorimetry. They are well based and have stood the test of time. Do that before trying to come up with an alternative theory of your own.
 
  • #46
Algr said:
I honestly do see all these colors as nearly equidistant
I'm not sure how you are concluding that the colours are 'equidistant'. The CIE Colour space diagram is the standard way of plotting the chrominance values of colours and the distance between to 'just perceptibly' different colours depends on where they are in colour space. The 'Macadam Diagram' in this link (In the Tolerance section) shows how our resolution between adjacent colours varies over colour space. Those Ellipses (scaled by a factor of 10, to make it more obvious) show that, in the greens area, we are much less fussy than in the reds and pinks area. We are particularly sensitive to differences in skin tones as we use that information to judge emotions. I guess that there are few examples of highly saturated green colours in nature so we do not need to be able to distinguish there. Leaves of different shades of green will all have desaturated colours, where our colour vision is more discriminating. The details must all be down to evolutionary advantages and cost.
Algr said:
if you kept trying to expand a spectrum further, you would simply end up with something too dim to see
Yes. That is absolutely correct. The narrower the bandwidth, the less energy is admitted (that applies to RF signals as well) and the resulting signal can become too low to 'measure' or detect. This is 'why' our light sensitive cells use very wide band analysis filtering. If you use a spectrometer to look at fine details of the spectrum of light from stars, you will be limited by the actual amount of light power that arrives in a very narrow range of wavelengths that fall on your detector (or film).
 
  • #47
The chart is my response to the "six colors" in the title of this thread. Take a look at the triangle at the bottom of the chart. Compare it to the square above and to the left. These are easily more different than red is from orange, and yet most people would have difficulty assigning names to them. They are both "Blue". We see one as darker than than other because our eyes aren't very sensitive to blue light. I'm well aware that yellow isn't really a primary (for additive color) but it seems to have a psychological identity so unlike its components. There is good reason why traffic lights are red-yellow-green, and not red-green-blue.

Apparently Isaac Newton would have called the primary color in the square "indigo" and the secondary in the triangle "blue". In modern language, the blue triangle is "Cyan".
 
Last edited:
  • Like
Likes rootone
  • #48
Algr said:
The chart is my response to the "six colors" in the title of this thread. Take a look at the triangle at the bottom of the chart. Compare it to the square above and to the left. These are easily more different than red is from orange, and yet most people would have difficulty assigning names to them. They are both "Blue". We see one as darker than than other because our eyes aren't very sensitive to blue light. I'm well aware that yellow isn't really a primary (for additive color) but it seems to have a psychological identity so unlike its components. There is good reason why traffic lights are red-yellow-green, and not red-green-blue.

Apparently Isaac Newton would have called the primary color in the square "indigo" and the secondary in the triangle "blue". In modern language, the blue triangle is "Cyan".
Like I already said, you should try to read some stuff on modern colorimetry (1930's actually) and on the tristimulus theory of colour vision. (Google and learn) There is loads of information about this theory which is the basis of colour TV and film. TV colour fidelity is pretty damn good these days so your theory will need to be a bit more than one diagram with coloured squares and triangles. You need to account for the response of the eye to pale brown, khaki and the colour of facial skin as well. It is not a trivial subject.
 
  • #49
I don't really have a "theory" these are just observations.
 
  • #50
Algr said:
I don't really have a "theory" these are just observations.
The diagram you presented implies that you have tried to systematise what you have observed. The CIE colour diagram has a place for all the colours you (we) experience and shows how they fit into a universal system that works well. You are right to say that many nontechnical but highly creative users of colour use their own naive (I use the term in a non-judgemental way) models. I know that approach can produce excellent results, despite its technical flaws and they have been doing it for thousands of years.
I still recommend that you look into the present state of knowledge of Colorimetry. It could help you with reconciling your subjective observations with the accepted system. It may involve some brain ache, though! :smile:
 
Last edited:
  • Like
Likes rootone and davenn
  • #51
rootone said:
That gives six possible combinations that our visual cortex can process, rg, rb, gr, gb, bg, br, to associate a colour to an object.
I don't get this. The brain doesn't process in pairs like this.

It processes the relative values of all three types - simply: rgb.
 
  • Like
Likes Electron Spin
  • #52
In my current reality there are 3 primary colours..

But... That's just mine...

Hello Dave!
 
  • #53
DaveC426913 said:
I don't get this. The brain doesn't process in pairs like this.

It processes the relative values of all three types - simply: rgb.
I couldn't find that post from routone, for a suitable riposte and I have already suggested that he / everyone should do some reading about the pretty well established tristimulus theory of colour vision. This thread is not going anywhere whilst people are still posting with Primary School ideas, based on mixing paints and crayons. Personal theories, based on no evidence are discouraged on PF, surely.
Edit: I wondered whether this response is too grumpy but I have reconsidered it to be appropriate. :smile:
 
  • #54
I'm not proposing a personal theory,and I will look into the established theory you mentioned.
My earlier post was really just a stab at a guess as for why traditionally artists, (of greater talent than schoolkids) thought in terms of 3 primary colours.
3 primary colours and their possible simple combinations gives a total of six easily identifiable colours.
That idea does correspond to what is now known to be the physical nature of light receptor neurons (rod cells) in human eyes.
I do know of course that visible light is not intrinsically in 3 bands,and what we are talking of here is human perception, not the nature of light as such,
 
Last edited:
  • #55
rootone said:
My earlier post was really just a stab at a guess as for why traditionally artists, (of greater talent than schoolkids) thought in terms of 3 primary colours.
The 'Primaries' used by artists are not the RGB primaries, used in additive colour mixing. The whole system is different for producing the required range of colours because using pigments / paints / inks is subtractive. Very few artists try to paint with just three primaries (Cyan, Yellow and Magenta) because you can only produce saturated results (strong colours) by reducing the available luminance. One primary masks the other, reducing the total light available. If an artist (or printer* designer) wants to produce high fidelity and bright colours, it is necessary for them to use extra colours ('spot colours' are used for well known colours - such as the Coca-cola Red).
I can't understand why you insist on limiting your colorimetric rules to just six colours. The last time I experienced this limitation was in programming the BBC Microcomputer which had only binary values for its RGB video signals. People with normal colour vision can recognise and remember many more than your six colours and many colours are not part of the spectral set (Newton's six or seven). Why are you still defending this inadequate colour system? We normally try to work with appropriate accuracy in any other fields of Science and Engineering.
PS, it's the Cone receptors that are responsible for colour perception.
*Dot matrix printers tend to use interleaved coloured dots where possible so they can achieve high saturation without compromising too much on luminance.
 
  • #56
Your monitor is able to make 16 million colors. Of course this is using 3 colors varying in intensity (256 for each). In the human visible spectrum alone you can have virtually infinite. Now take into account you can mix colors together. But wait there's more: X-rays, Ultra violets, Gamma rays, Radio, Microwave, infra-red...
The problem is, our eyes have a limited range that they can distinguish, rendering many of the wavelengths redundant.
 
Last edited:
  • #57
DarkBabylon said:
rendering many of the wavelengths redundant.
No so much "redundant" as indistinguishable from their neighbours. All wavelengths contribute energy to what the colour sensors 'see'. The point has already been made that the ability to distinguish between adjacent colours will depend on the actual light level. It's the same old problem of Signal to Noise Ratio you need enough energy arriving on the three sensor to be able to assign a 'value' to their response so that your brain can do the sums and come up with a definite colour.
The Cones need much higher incident light levels in order to do this than the much more sensitive Rods. We always take clothes into a bright area to assess whether or not we have a good enough match. Daylight is best because the illuminant will affect the actual colour we decide we're seeing. CFL bulbs and old Fluo tubes are really bad for colour appreciation.
 
  • #58
The OP has long been answered. Time to close.
 
  • Like
Likes davenn

Similar threads

Replies
7
Views
1K
  • Classical Physics
Replies
21
Views
916
  • Programming and Computer Science
Replies
1
Views
551
  • Engineering and Comp Sci Homework Help
Replies
4
Views
863
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
Replies
4
Views
1K
  • Special and General Relativity
Replies
25
Views
860
  • Other Physics Topics
Replies
2
Views
2K
Replies
7
Views
4K
  • Special and General Relativity
Replies
4
Views
781
Back
Top