Can dogs accurately detect cancer in humans through smell?

In summary, these trained dogs can be very accurate at detecting lung cancer in the breath of cancer sufferers. They need further study to be sure that the dogs are actually detecting cancer, and the results may be too good to be true.
  • #1
Ivan Seeking
Staff Emeritus
Science Advisor
Gold Member
8,142
1,755
SAN RAFAEL, Calif., Jan. 5 (UPI) -- Researchers say dogs can be trained to detect both early and late stage lung and breast cancer in humans.

A study to be published in the March 2006 issue of the journal Integrative Cancer Therapies says dogs may have the capacity to contribute to early cancer detection.

...Subsequent studies confirmed the ability of trained dogs to detect both melanomas and bladder cancers [continued]
http://www.upi.com/ConsumerHealthDaily/view.php?StoryID=20060105-102420-5522r
 
Biology news on Phys.org
  • #2
:bugeye: For sure these trained dogs are more expensive!
 
  • #3
Hi,

Dogs are kwnon as our best friends.
But it works unfortunately only for few.
 
  • #4
They already use rats for their nose, I'm not sure what disease it was, TBC or cancer.
 
  • #5
so how do you smell cancer? Don't the cells smell the same as normal ones?
 
  • #6
Ah, here's the answer:

Tomorrow's *New York Times* includes an article: "Dogs Excel on Smell Test
to Find Cancer" by Donald McNeil.

Here's the article:

In the small world of people who train dogs to sniff cancer, a little-known
Northern California clinic has made a big claim: that it has trained five
dogs - three Labradors and two Portuguese water dogs - to detect lung cancer
in the breath of cancer sufferers with 99 percent accuracy.

The study was based on well-established concepts. It has been known since
the 80's that tumors exude tiny amounts of alkanes and benzene derivatives
not found in healthy tissue.

Other researchers have shown that dogs, whose noses can pick up odors in the
low parts-per-billion range, can be trained to detect skin cancers or react
differently to dried urine from healthy people and those with bladder
cancer, but never with such remarkable consistency.

The near-perfection in the clinic's study, as Dr. Donald Berry, the chairman
of biostatistics at M. D. Anderson Cancer Center in Houston, put it, "is off
the charts: there are no laboratory tests as good as this, not Pap tests,
not diabetes tests, nothing."

As a result, he and other cancer experts say they are skeptical, but
intrigued. Michael McCulloch, research director for the Pine Street
Foundation in Marin County, Calif., and the lead researcher on the study,
acknowledged that the results seemed too good to be true. (For breast
cancer, with a smaller number of samples, the dogs were right about 88
percent of the time with almost no false positives, which compares favorably
to mammograms.)

"Yes, we were astounded, as well," Mr. McCulloch said. "And that's why it
needs to be replicated with other dogs, plus chemical analysis of what's in
the breath."

He is applying for National Science Foundation grants to try just that, he
said. The fact that the study was carried out by a clinic supported by the
Pine Street Foundation that combines traditional chemotherapy with
acupuncture and herbal medicine raised suspicions, as did the fact that it
is to be published by a little-known journal, Integrative Cancer Therapies.
(The journal published it online last year.)

But experts who read the study could not find any obvious fatal flaw in its
methodology, and the idea that dogs can detect cancer is "not crazy at all,"
said Dr. Ted Gansler, director of medical content in health information for
the American Cancer Society. "It's biologically plausible," he said, "but
there has to be a lot more study and confirmation of effectiveness."

Dr. Berry, too, was interested but suspicious. "If true, it's huge," he
said. "Which is one reason to be skeptical."

Dr. Berry noted, half-jokingly, that Gregor Mendel, the 19th-century
discoverer of the laws of genetics, also reported data on his crossbreeding
of green and yellow peas that was too good to be true: he repeatedly came up
with the perfect 3-1 ratios he predicted. "But we've forgiven Mendel and his
gardener," Dr. Berry added, "because his theory turned out to be right."

In Mr. McCulloch's study, the five dogs, borrowed from owners and Guide Dogs
for the Blind, were trained as if detecting bombs. They repeatedly heard a
clicker and got a treat when they found a desired odor in many identical
smelling spots.

The clinic collected breath samples in plastic tubes filled with
polypropylene wool from 55 people just after biopsies found lung cancer and
from 31 patients with breast cancer, as well as from 83 healthy volunteers.

The tubes were numbered, and then placed in plastic boxes and presented to
the dogs, five at a time. If the dog smelled cancer, it was supposed to sit.

For breath from lung cancer patients, Mr. McCulloch reported, the dogs
correctly sat 564 times and incorrectly 10 times. (By adjusting for other
factors, the researchers determined the accuracy rate at 99 percent.)

For the breath from healthy patients, they sat 4 times and did not sit 708
times.

Experts who read the study raised various objections: The smells of
chemotherapy or smoking would be clues, they said. Or the healthy breath
samples could have been collected in a different room on different days. Or
the dogs could pick up subtle cues - like the tiny, unintentional movements
of observers picked up by Clever Hans, the 19th-century "counting horse," as
he neared a correct answer. But Mr. McCulloch said cancer patients who had
begun chemotherapy were excluded, smokers were included in both groups and
the breath samples were collected in the same rooms on the same days. The
tubes were numbered elsewhere, he said, and the only assistant who knew
which samples were cancerous was out of the room while the dogs were
working.

"The fact that dogs did this is kind of beside the point," he said. "What
this proved is that there are detectable differences in the breath of cancer
patients. Now technology has to rise to that challenge."

The next step, he said, will be to analyze breath samples with a gas
chromatograph to figure out exactly which mixes of chemicals the dogs are
reacting to.

Even if the dogs are accurate in repeat experiments, Dr. Gansler of the
American Cancer Society said, it will be useful only as a preliminary scan.
"It's not like someone would start chemotherapy based on a dog test," he
said. "They'd still get a biopsy."
 

1. How can dogs detect cancer?

Dogs have a highly sensitive sense of smell, with their noses containing up to 300 million scent receptors compared to humans' 5 million. This allows them to detect subtle changes in the chemicals present in a person's body, including the compounds produced by cancer cells.

2. Is there scientific evidence to support the claim that dogs can smell cancer?

Yes, there have been numerous studies conducted on the ability of dogs to detect cancer in humans. One study published in the Journal of Urology found that trained dogs were able to correctly identify prostate cancer samples with 98% accuracy.

3. How accurate are dogs at detecting cancer?

The accuracy of dogs in detecting cancer varies depending on the type of cancer, the training of the dog, and the conditions of the test. However, studies have shown that trained dogs have a high success rate in detecting various types of cancer, with some studies reporting accuracy rates of up to 99%.

4. Can dogs be trained to detect other diseases besides cancer?

Yes, dogs have been trained to detect various diseases including diabetes, Parkinson's disease, and malaria. Their incredible sense of smell allows them to detect subtle changes in the body that may indicate the presence of a disease.

5. Can dogs be used as a reliable screening method for cancer?

While dogs have shown promising results in detecting cancer, they are not currently used as a reliable screening method. More research is needed to fully understand the capabilities of dogs in detecting cancer and to develop standardized training protocols. Additionally, other factors such as the cost and accessibility of using dogs as a screening method would need to be considered.

Back
Top