Counterintuitive Problem with HTH patterns

  • B
  • Thread starter TheCanadian
  • Start date
  • Tags
    Patterns
In summary, The conversation is about an exercise in David Williams' book where the participants discuss a strategy for winning in a game with transition diagrams. The hardest patterns to beat are HTH and THT, but HHH and TTT cannot beat anything. The conversation also includes a correction and some second thoughts about the best strategy, but ultimately the participants come up with a solution that has a 2/3 probability of winning, except in the cases of HHH and TTT where the probability is 7/8.
  • #1
TheCanadian
367
13
Screen Shot 2016-12-12 at 9.50.47 PM.png
Screen Shot 2016-12-12 at 9.50.55 PM.png


Just came across this exercise in David Williams' book and thought it was worth sharing.
 
  • Like
Likes Stephen Tashi
Physics news on Phys.org
  • #2
TheCanadian said:
View attachment 110343 View attachment 110344

Just came across this exercise in David Williams' book and thought it was worth sharing.
Yes, very interesting. I could not find an easy way to figure it out ... lots of little transition diagrams.
The hardest ones to beat are HTH and THT. In each case there is only one choice.
HHH and TTT do not beat anything.
 
  • #3
haruspex said:
Yes, very interesting. I could not find an easy way to figure it out ... lots of little transition diagrams.
The hardest ones to beat are HTH and THT. In each case there is only one choice.
HHH and TTT do not beat anything.
CORRECTION: THIS POST IS WRONG (see the next two posts)
This is talking about the first occurrence of those patterns in a long string, right? I think that as long as your last two letters match his first two and your first letter is different from your second (his first), you string has strictly better odds of occurring first. It doesn't matter what his last letter is.
If he picks HTH, you pick THT. If he picks THT, you pick HTH. You will have better odds. His first occurrence is matched in probability by your choice occurring just before his. ( T HTH gives you a win in the first case and H THT gives you a win in the second case. )
 
Last edited:
  • #4
FactChecker said:
This is talking about the first occurrence of those patterns in a long string, right? I think that as long as your last two letters match his first two and your first letter is different from your second (his first), you string has strictly better odds of occurring first. It doesn't matter what his last letter is.
No, that does not work. If she chooses HTH then your algorithm chooses THT, but by symmetry those are equal. To win you must choose HHT.
 
  • Like
Likes FactChecker
  • #5
haruspex said:
No, that does not work. If she chooses HTH then your algorithm chooses THT, but by symmetry those are equal. To win you must choose HHT.
CORRECTION: I am having second thoughts about this post (see edit below)
(I added some to my prior post.) Looking only at the first occurrence of his pick, 'HTH', your pick of 'THT' will have equal odds and will come before in the string 'T HTH'. They are not symmetric cases since you have guaranteed yourself a win in a small subset that he needed for a win.

EDIT: I see your point. It is symmetric, since his 'HTH' has the same advantage of coming before my first 'THT' in the string 'H THT'. So your solution is to copy the first two characters of his as your second and third and to make your first character different from his second character (your third char). That would make it asymmetric and give you an advantage.
 
Last edited:
  • #6
FactChecker said:
copy the first two characters of his as your second and third and to make your first character different from his second character (your third char)
Yes, that seems to work. Haven't figured out whether that gives the best advantage in each case.
 
  • Like
Likes FactChecker
  • #7
TheCanadian said:
View attachment 110343 View attachment 110344

Just came across this exercise in David Williams' book and thought it was worth sharing.
In fact I can choose one such that probability of my winning is 2/3, except in the cases where you selected HHH or TTT, then my chances are 7/8.
When I was a student back in the 50s I won my food money with this and the Monte Hall trick.
The cute part is that A beats B beats C beats D beats A. Winning isn't transitive.
 
  • #8
FactChecker said:
CORRECTION: I am having second thoughts about this post (see edit below)
(I added some to my prior post.) Looking only at the first occurrence of his pick, 'HTH', your pick of 'THT' will have equal odds and will come before in the string 'T HTH'. They are not symmetric cases since you have guaranteed yourself a win in a small subset that he needed for a win.

EDIT: I see your point. It is symmetric, since his 'HTH' has the same advantage of coming before my first 'THT' in the string 'H THT'. So your solution is to copy the first two characters of his as your second and third and to make your first character different from his second character (your third char). That would make it asymmetric and give you an advantage.
EDIT2: This is wrong again. It can end up with your string being the same as his. I give up as far as stating a simple solution that works for all cases.
 
  • #9
Zafa Pi said:
In fact I can choose one such that probability of my winning is 2/3, except in the cases where you selected HHH or TTT, then my chances are 7/8.
I can even do better. Let's list the 4 cases with more Ts than Hs in alphabetical order (the other cases are symmetric):
HTT is beat by HHT with probability 2/3.
THT is beat by TTH with probability 2/3.
TTH is beat by HTT with probability 3/4.
TTT is beat by HTT with probability 7/8.

The proof of the 1st case: Let p = the probability that HHT beats HTT. We must eventually start with H in either case.
If the next is H (probability 1/2) then HHT wins.
If the next is T (probability 1/2) then !/2 the time HHT loses and 1/2 the time we start over. So p = 1/2 + 1/2*(1/2*0 + 1/2*p), thus p = 2/3.

The 2nd case has the same argument. The 3rd and 4th cases are easier.
 
  • Like
Likes haruspex
  • #10
FactChecker said:
EDIT2: This is wrong again. It can end up with your string being the same as his. I give up as far as stating a simple solution that works for all cases.
No, I think it works. Without loss of generality, he chose a sequence starting with H. Your rule gives:
Code:
His    Yours
HHH  THH
HHT  THH
HTH  HHT
HTT  HHT

Generalising to strings length N>2, let his choice be (c1, .. , cN). For convenience, abbreviate c1, .. , cN-1 to S.
So he chose (S, cN). You choose (c0, S), where c0 is not c2.
There are equal probabilities that the first N tosses will produce one of the two sequences. Otherwise, let your probability of winning be p.
To reach a decision, the sequence S has to arise. There is an evens chance it was preceded by c0, making you the winner, and an evens chance that it will not be followed by cN, giving you another chance. The rule for choosing c0 prevents that argument working symmetrically.
This appears to give p = 1/2+p/4, so p = 2/3.
But here's the problem: If S is not followed by cN in the throw sequence then we might nevertheless still be in a partial match with either chosen sequence. E.g. if his sequence is HTHH and the tosses produce HTHT, that last HT puts him back to a half-way match. Our sequence, HHTH, is doing even better, but is that generally true?
 
  • Like
Likes FactChecker
  • #11
haruspex said:
No, I think it works. Without loss of generality, he chose a sequence starting with H. Your rule gives:
Code:
His    Yours
HHH  THH
HHT  THH
HTH  HHT
HTT  HHT
Oh! Thanks! I got myself confused.
 

FAQ: Counterintuitive Problem with HTH patterns

What is a counterintuitive problem with HTH patterns?

A counterintuitive problem with HTH patterns refers to a situation or concept that goes against our natural instincts or common sense when interpreting data or patterns in high-throughput (HTH) experiments. It challenges our initial assumptions and expectations and may lead to unexpected results or conclusions.

How can a counterintuitive problem with HTH patterns occur?

A counterintuitive problem with HTH patterns can occur due to various reasons, such as biased data collection methods, incomplete or incorrect data analysis, or complex underlying relationships between variables that are not easily observable or understood.

What are some examples of counterintuitive problems with HTH patterns?

One example of a counterintuitive problem with HTH patterns is the Simpson's paradox, where a trend appears in different subgroups of data but disappears or reverses when these subgroups are combined. Another example is the "gambler's fallacy," where people believe that past outcomes can affect future outcomes in a random process, even though the two are statistically independent.

How can scientists address counterintuitive problems with HTH patterns?

To address counterintuitive problems with HTH patterns, scientists can use rigorous and unbiased data collection and analysis methods, carefully consider potential confounding factors, and replicate experiments to ensure the validity and generalizability of their findings. They can also consult with experts in statistics or other relevant fields to gain a deeper understanding of the data and interpret the results more accurately.

Why is it important for scientists to be aware of counterintuitive problems with HTH patterns?

Being aware of counterintuitive problems with HTH patterns is crucial for scientists because it allows them to avoid making incorrect conclusions or decisions based on misleading or incomplete data. It also promotes critical thinking and a deeper understanding of the complexities and limitations of scientific research, leading to more accurate and reliable findings.

Similar threads

Back
Top