Experiment with smaller children

In summary, there have been experiments conducted with smaller children and adults to determine their ability to accurately estimate the volume of liquid in different shaped containers. These experiments have shown that there is a tendency to overestimate the volume in shorter, wider containers, even for experienced bartenders. This could be due to a visual illusion where the height dimension is given more weight in determining volume. However, there is no clear explanation for this phenomenon. More research is needed to fully understand how the visual system processes information and how we perceive volume.
  • #1
WackyTaffy
6
0
There has been an experiment with smaller children. First, there was a smaller container which had a certain amount of liquid (Let's say 355 mL, the same as a coke can). Then, there was a taller glass, with the same amount. Without saying which had more, they were asked which one had more liquid in it. They chose the taller glass.

There was also some experiment where they had to guess which one had more liquid, and then had to pour it from one size glass to another.

I forgot the experiment exactly, but I still feel this way with certain things. Why?
For example, Jones Co Soda (The best soda company in the world, in my opinion) makes glass bottles, 355 mL each, with a resealable lid. Now, coke cans have the same amount of liquid. Yet I swear, it seems like the Jones Co ones have more. Is this because of the illusion that it being taller it has more liquid?
 
Biology news on Phys.org
  • #2
This clearly involves some kind of visual illusion or another. Probably what is going on is that the visual system processes information in such a way as to implicitly assume that taller things have greater volume than wider things (all other things being more or less equal). Another way of saying that might go as follows: if there exists some perceptual estimate of volume based on an object's perceived height, length, and width, the visual system might be inherently biased to give the height dimension more weight to its volume estimate than it does to the length or width dimensions.

For a more detailed and qualified explanation, you should try to track down the paper describing this study. If you can find the article reference but can't view the paper online, I'd be willing to access the article myself and see what their explanation of this effect is.
 
  • #3
As I recall Piaget did a lot of experiments like this, and hypothesized that the ability to estimate the relative amount of liquid in different shaped containers, which he held to require a certain level of abstraction, didn't develop until the children were older. Piaget's theories have had a lot of influence on educational thought in the USA.
 
  • #4
I haven't found the original paper, but here's a new article (Dec 2005) discussing essentially the same effect: Shape of glass and amount of alcohol poured: comparative study of effect of practice and concentration.

Results: Aiming to pour a "shot" of alcohol (1.5 ounces, 44.3 ml), both students and bartenders poured more into short, wide glasses than into tall slender glasses (46.1 ml v 44.7 ml and 54.6 ml v 46.4 ml, respectively). Practice reduced the tendency to overpour, but not for short, wide glasses. Despite an average of six years of experience, bartenders poured 20.5% more into short, wide glasses than tall, slender ones; paying careful attention reduced but did not eliminate the effect.
So it looks as if learning can improve our ability to estimate volumes of various shapes, but even as adults with extensive experience we still tend to be fooled by certain shapes (specifically, our tendency to underestimate the volume of short, wide containers seems particularly resistant to learning). The authors of this paper don't propose any explanation for why this occurs though.
 
Last edited:
  • #5
hypnagogue said:
This clearly involves some kind of visual illusion or another. Probably what is going on is that the visual system processes information in such a way as to implicitly assume that taller things have greater volume than wider things (all other things being more or less equal).
Yes. Hold that thought...
hypnagogue said:
Another way of saying that might go as follows: if there exists some perceptual estimate of volume based on an object's perceived height, length, and width, the visual system might be inherently biased to give the height dimension more weight to its volume estimate than it does to the length or width dimensions.
Note that, from a single viewpoint, we see objects as areas. Volume is only implied.

A round glass that is twice as tall really does fill twice as much of our vision. A round glass that is shorter but fatter does not actually fill twice as much of our vision - it only fills root(2) as much. We can't explicitly see the added front-to-back depth.

Really, the experiment is cheating in a sense. A truly objective experiment would use two glasses that have identical front-to-back depths.
 
  • #6
Demonstration:

2 ROUND glasses, each holds 1 litre

Glass A:
height: ~16cm
Thus, width, depth = ~8cm


Glass B:
We give it a height = half of glass A, so: ~8cm
Thus, width,depth = ~5cm (to hold same volume as glass A)

Note that Glass A (128cm^2) actually DOES look smaller than glass B (40cm^2).

A PROPER glass B would be:
Height: 8cm
Width: ~16cm
Depth: ~8cm (same as Glass A)

In a proper experiment, Glass B would be much wider than it is.
 
Last edited:
  • #7
Hypnagogue said:
So it looks as if learning can improve our ability to estimate volumes of various shapes, but even as adults with extensive experience we still tend to be fooled by certain shapes (specifically, our tendency to underestimate the volume of short, wide containers seems particularly resistant to learning). The authors of this paper don't propose any explanation for why this occurs though.
When I worked at a coffee house I generally measured out what I poured into containers with an imaginary mark off point because I was familiar with the containers and approximatly where the proper amount should fill them to. I don't remember having ever tried to observe the actual volume itself.
This may be part of what happened with the bartenders but I think DaveC has a better theory of the overall issue.
 

1. What are the ethical considerations when conducting experiments with children?

When conducting experiments with children, it is important to obtain informed consent from both the child and their parent or legal guardian. It is also necessary to ensure that the experiment is age-appropriate and does not cause any harm or distress to the child. Researchers must also follow strict protocols for protecting the privacy and confidentiality of the child's personal information.

2. How do you ensure the safety of children during an experiment?

The safety of children is of utmost importance when conducting experiments. Researchers must carefully design the experiment to minimize any potential risks or harm to the child. They must also closely supervise the child during the experiment and have a plan in place for addressing any potential emergencies. It is also important to obtain parental consent and keep them informed throughout the experiment.

3. What are the benefits of conducting experiments with children?

Conducting experiments with children can provide valuable insights into child development and help researchers better understand how children learn and think. It can also lead to the development of new interventions or treatments for childhood issues and disorders. In addition, involving children in research can empower them and give them a voice in the scientific community.

4. How do you address the potential for bias in experiments with children?

Bias can be a concern in any research study, including those involving children. To address this, researchers must carefully design the experiment to minimize any potential biases. This may include using control groups, random assignment, and blind studies. It is also important for researchers to be aware of their own biases and take steps to mitigate their influence on the study.

5. What are some best practices for communicating with children during an experiment?

When interacting with children during an experiment, it is important to use age-appropriate language and avoid using jargon or technical terms. Researchers should also be patient and allow the child to ask questions or express their thoughts. It is also important to maintain a positive and encouraging attitude to make the child feel comfortable and engaged in the experiment.

Similar threads

Replies
1
Views
1K
  • General Engineering
Replies
1
Views
881
  • High Energy, Nuclear, Particle Physics
Replies
12
Views
2K
  • STEM Educators and Teaching
Replies
4
Views
2K
  • Introductory Physics Homework Help
Replies
12
Views
1K
Replies
8
Views
2K
Replies
14
Views
2K
Replies
10
Views
1K
  • Introductory Physics Homework Help
Replies
21
Views
5K
Back
Top