sysprog said:
It's an 'if then else' statement ##-## you don't need to know where the car is to recognize that the statement is true.
Okay - the condition in the "If...Then...Else" statement cannot be evaluated by the contestant. The statement is indeed true, but it cannot be used by the contestant because it ignores information she possesses that can change the implied answer, and suggests she should decide based on information she does not possess.
Again - the chances for the original choice can change, based on information that is in her possession. The fact that they don't change under the most reasonable application of that information does not mean they can't change.
These two problems - Monty Hall and Two Children - have probably done more to teach incorrect conditional probability methodology to students than any other. The correct methods are not that difficult to comprehend, but they do require attention to details that sometimes seem unnecessary.
- Create a table of every outcome that is initially possible, even those you later find out did not occur.
- Assign a probability to each.
- Remove the outcomes that you later learn did not happen.
- Add up the remaining probabilities.
- Divide these remaining probabilities by that sum, so they again add up to 1.
In the Monty Hall Problem, many students who get the wrong answer will do a better job of following these steps than most teachers who get the right answer. They say (assuming the initial choice is 31 and #3 is opened):
- The car could be behind #1, #2, or #3.
- Each has a probability of 1/3.
- But door #3 does not have the car, so remove it.
- The remaining probabilities sum to 2/3.
- So each now becomes (1/3)/(2/3)=1/2.
The error is that there are more possible outcomes. This assumes the Host must open door #3 if it has a goat. The correct solution recognizes that which door was opened is part of the information the contestant possesses.
- The combinations for (Car,Open) are (1,2), (1,3), (2,3), and (3,2). (Note that #1 can't be opened.)
- The corresponding probabilities are 1/6, 1/6, 1/3, and 1/3.
- But, for example, door #3 was opened so two of these are removed. That leaves (1,3) and (2,3).
- The corresponding probabilities are 1/6 and 1/3, which sum to 1/2.
- The probability for (1,3) is now (1/6)/(1/3)=1/2, and for (2,3) is now (1/3)/(1/2)=2/3.
Any other solution only serves to confuse students.
In the Two Child Problem:
- There are six possible outcomes, that include hich gender is the one you learn is included in the family; that is, (Older, Younger; Learn). They are (B,B;B), (B,G;B), (B,G;G), (G,B;B), (G,B;G), and (G,G;G).
- The probabilities are 1/4, 1/8, 1/8, 1/8, 1/8, and 1/4.
- You learned about a boy, so only three remain: (B,B;B), (B,G;B), and G,B;B).
- The corresponding probabilities are 1/4, 1/8, and 1/8 which sum to 1/2.
- The updated probabilities are now 1/2, 1/4, and 1/4.
Many will disagree with step #2 here, claiming that the probabilities are 1/4,1/4,0,1/4,0, and 1/4. That is the same as when you assume door #3 must be opened in the MHP, and so getting the answer 1/2.