How does context help us understand language?

  • Thread starter Thread starter Jimmy Snyder
  • Start date Start date
  • Tags Tags
    Jump
AI Thread Summary
A person takes the elevator to the 86th floor of the Empire State Building, jumps off, and freefalls to the street below, yet walks away uninjured. The discussion revolves around the interpretation of the scenario, with participants debating whether the wording implies a literal jump from the building or if it could refer to a jump from a lower point, such as the curb. Some suggest that the fall could be survivable due to factors like landing techniques or safety equipment, while others argue that the phrasing creates ambiguity and leads to misconceptions about the situation. The conversation also touches on grammatical nuances and reference errors in the context of the puzzle, with participants exploring how language can mislead interpretations. Ultimately, the crux of the debate lies in the cleverness of the puzzle's wording and the assumptions made by those attempting to solve it.
Jimmy Snyder
Messages
1,122
Reaction score
22
A person takes the elevator to the 86th floor of the Empire State Building, jumps off and freefalls all the way to the street below. He walks away uninjured. How can this be?
 
Physics news on Phys.org
He's superman?

OOH! he's nugie jumping! No, freefall

is he any sort of superhero?
 
He must have reached terminal velocity at about 120 mph. A fall from 100ft isn't much different from 1000ft. That someone could walk away after a fall like that is incredible, but I've heard stories of people falling from planes and surviving. Coincidence, luck, whatever you want to call it, I wouldn't try to repeat that experiment.
 
The person jumping was a "she"; the "he" was the pedestrian she narrowly missed..[/color]
 
Is it the same person who jumps from the 86-th storey the one who walks away unharmed?..

Daniel.
 
He could land on one of those huge balloons that are designed to be fell on from large heights.
 
It was the same person who did all those things. There was no balloon to break his fall and he landed standing up so that the full force of the impact traveled up his entire body. He didn't even fall over.

Hint. I haven't told you everything that he did
 
He reduced g?
 
Some other things he did at about that time: (don't read this if you still want to solve the puzzle)

1. He took the elevator back down.

2. He stood on the steps of the building.

3. He smoked a cigarette (he's quite the daredevil).

4. He walked over to the curb.
 
Last edited:
  • #10
Blaargh, I think I've got it: He jumps up and down inside the elevator on its way down..[/color]
 
  • #11
Does he travel back down to a lower floor before jumping?[/color]
Okay, I checked. Yeah, I rock. ;)
 
  • #12
It is unfair that you don't tell us everything he does, in sequence. For instance, I could ask you, "A person loads a .45 JHP round into a pistol and shoots himself in the head with that pistol. He walks away uninjured." And the trick would be that he loads the round, then takes it out and replaces it with a blank.
 
  • #13
If that's the intended answer, the problem is based around a reference error. "Jumps off" implies that he jumps off the 86th floor, not off the curb.

Virtually any question can be answered in that way. Try the same idea on the "nothing" puzzler:
It's better than your favorite food, but poor people are more like to have it than rich people. If they eat it, though, they die. What is it?
The first "it" in the first sentence is diamonds. The second "it" in the first sentence is diabetes. The "it" in the second sentence is poisonous mushrooms. The "it" in the third sentence, and the "answer" to the puzzle, is Barney the purple dinosaur.
 
  • #14
Icebreaker said:
It is unfair that you don't tell us everything he does, in sequence.

A person takes the elevator to the 86th floor of the Empire State Building.
He gets off the elevator. He walks around. He puts a quarter into the binoculars and looks at the city. He smokes a cigarette. He chats with another tourist. He looks at his watch and frowns. He climbs over the security fence and dangles precariously over the edge.

How am I doing so far? I know I left out a lot of things. For instance, he lit his cigarette. But I'm certain I didn't leave out anything pertinent to the solution of the puzzle.

He jumps off the building and freefalls all the way to the street below. He walks away uninjured. How can this be?
 
  • #15
Oh, I know! He's juggling three balls while freefalling. :wink:
Or... he's dreaming.
 
  • #16
Godzilla? His other day job is an action performer. He does stunts like this all the time.
 
  • #17
jimmysnyder said:
How am I doing so far? I know I left out a lot of things. For instance, he lit his cigarette. But I'm certain I didn't leave out anything pertinent to the solution of the puzzle.

He jumps off the building and freefalls all the way to the street below. He walks away uninjured. How can this be?

You left the part where he took the elevator back down, as stated here:

jimmysnyder said:
Some other things he did at about that time: (don't read this if you still want to solve the puzzle)

1. He took the elevator back down

and here:

jimmysnyder said:
Hint. I haven't told you everything that he did
 
Last edited by a moderator:
  • #18
It's not even a matter of leaving things out, it's just a grammatical error. When the man "jumps off," the word "off" is an adverbial phrase with "it" implied. i.e., he "jumps off it." In this case, "it" means "the 86th floor of the Empire state building." Saying it refers to something else is just a grammatical error, specifically a reference error.

If you're going to violate grammar like that, why even bother saying he goes down to the bottom floor? Why not say that the jump referred to is a jump off a bus that the man did when he was five years old?

This was a bad question. Most of your questions are good but this one was not good.
 
  • #19
Icebreaker said:
You left the part where he took the elevator back down.
A person takes the elevator to the 86th floor of the Empire State Building.
He takes the elevator back down. He stands on the steps at the front of the building. He jumps off the building. He smokes a cigarette. He walks over to the curb and jumps into the street. He freefalls all the way to the street below. He walks away uninjured. How can this be?

I don't get the point of a puzzle in which everything is stated explicitly. If you don't leave something out, it's not a puzzle. The point of this puzzle is no different from many others. Something is left out. The wording deliberately leads you to ignore that fact. I note that someone solved this 'unfair' puzzle. How do you explain his ability to think outside this particular box?
 
  • #20
BicycleTree said:
It's not even a matter of leaving things out, it's just a grammatical error. When the man "jumps off," the word "off" is an adverbial phrase with "it" implied. i.e., he "jumps off it." In this case, "it" means "the 86th floor of the Empire state building." Saying it refers to something else is just a grammatical error, specifically a reference error.
A person takes the elevator to the 86th floor of the Empire State Building, jumps off the Empire State Building, freefalls all the way to the street below. He walks away from it uninjured. How can this be?

The 'grammatical error' is gone. Now what say you?
 
Last edited:
  • #21
Now it's a good question, though the answer is now a little too obvious.
 
  • #22
BicycleTree said:
Now it's a good question, though the answer is now a little too obvious.
Tough crowd. I think the puzzle is fine as originally stated. If you disagree, you have to explain how honestrosewater was able to figure it out.
 
  • #23
Because she was looking for tricks in the wording of the problem. Just because you made a mistake doesn't mean she can't make the same mistake. (no offense)
 
  • #24
jimmysnyder said:
Tough crowd. I think the puzzle is fine as originally stated. If you disagree, you have to explain how honestrosewater was able to figure it out.

Maybe something like:

"A person takes the elevator to the 86th floor of the Empire State Building to see the view, later he jumps off the building and freefalls all the way to the street below. He walks away uninjured. How can this be?"

That works; but then it's a little more obvious that it happened after some changes were made.
 
  • #25
You could just say "then jumps off the building." (you don't have to say it was "later" to make the puzzle grammatical, but your idea of saying "the building" is good to make the puzzle less obvious while still grammatical) So it would be:
A person takes the elevator to the 86th floor of the Empire State Building, then jumps off the building. He freefalls all the way to the street below. He walks away from it uninjured. How can this be?
 
  • #26
I think "A person takes the elevator to the 86th floor of the Empire State Building, jumps off and freefalls all the way to the street below." is structurally ambiguous. Why not assume he jumped off of the elevator? And recall the fallacy: x follows y does not imply x was caused by y. Well, maybe that's a bit of a stretch- but it is just for fun after all. I think some puzzles work by being ambiguous and tricking you into making the wrong assumptions. BTW, I looked at the "I didn't tell you everything" hint. :redface:
 
  • #27
It's not ambiguous. Any sentence of the form "I went to x and jumped off" is equivalent (just by grammar) to "I went to x and jumped off it" and is therefore equivalent to "I went to x and jumped off x."
 
  • #28
BicycleTree said:
It's not ambiguous. Any sentence of the form "I went to x and jumped off" is equivalent (just by grammar) to "I went to x and jumped off it" and is therefore equivalent to "I went to x and jumped off x."
Well, since the setting isn't formal, I wasn't being strict. But if you want to be strict, if you add it to get:

A person takes the elevator to the 86th floor of the Empire State Building, jumps off it...

What rule determines whether it refers to elevator, floor, Empire State Building, or something not even in the sentence? I've never seen such a rule, but I'm just now learning more about English grammar. I don't know the reasons why (I haven't studied pronoun reference yet), but from what I've read, the example above is indeed ambiguous, because it can refer to elevator, floor, or Empire State Building (or possibly something else). Googling "pronoun reference" turns up several examples.
 
  • #29
The pronoun references the most recently used noun that fits it. The most recently used noun is "the 86th floor." "of the Empire State Building" is only an adjective phrase. It's essentially part of the other noun ("the 86th floor"), so it can't be referenced on its own. "The 86th floor of the Empire State Building" is a single noun.
 
  • #30
When I use a word, it means exactly what I intend it to mean, no more, no less.
 
  • #31
All I can say to that is, frambaztadimble.
 
  • #32
a language forum ?
 
  • #33
BicycleTree said:
The pronoun references the most recently used noun that fits it.
Do you have a reference? I also wonder how that rule would handle this:

Mary and Martha took her children to the park.

The most recently used noun is "the 86th floor." "of the Empire State Building" is only an adjective phrase. It's essentially part of the other noun ("the 86th floor"), so it can't be referenced on its own. "The 86th floor of the Empire State Building" is a single noun.
Hm, it seems some define adjective phrases as any phrase that modifies a noun, while others define them as phrases whose heads are adjectives- a functional vs. formal thing. I use the latter. There are probably slightly different ways of parsing sentences, but here's how I would parse just the phrases (NP = noun phrase, VP = verb phrase, etc.):

[NP A person] [VP takes] [NP the elevator [PP to [NP the 86th floor [PP of [NP the Empire State Building]]]]]...

So the head of the most recent NP is elevator. You can see the others are not needed:

A person takes the elevator.

I don't see why to the 86th floor and of the Empire State Building would be treated differently. They are both prepositional phrases to me. Edit: But you may not distinguish prepositional phrases?
 
Last edited:
  • #34
ArielGenesis said:
a language forum ?
That would be great! When I start seriously studying language, I'm going to try to stir up enough interest to warrant a linguistics forum. Is there enough interest already??
 
  • #35
What, precisely, do you mean by the term 'linguistics'?
 
  • #36
jimmysnyder said:
What, precisely, do you mean by the term 'linguistics'?
The study of language. It's a very broad field. ;)
 
  • #37
Adjective phrases and adverbial phrases are types of prepositional phrases that act as adjectives or adverbs.

"Mary and Martha took her children to the park" is truly a reference error. There is a preference for "Martha" but it is not clear whose children they are. There is also, however, a set of implicit rules for determining which reference is intended, which is the reason the vast majority of literate people--i.e. respected authors--make certain kinds of so-called "reference errors" continually yet nevertheless understand one another easily and unambiguously. I believe the reason some grammar textbooks deny this set of rules is for simplicity.

To demonstrate this, I searched for Charles Dickens on Google, and the first work of his I clicked on the first site that came up was A Christmas Carol. The first thing I read under that heading is his introduction:

Charles Dickens said:
I have endeavoured in this Ghostly little book,
to raise the Ghost of an Idea, which shall not
put my readers out of humour with themselves,
with each other, with the season, or with me.
May it haunt their houses pleasantly, and no
one wish to lay it.

Their faithful Friend and Servant, C. D. December, 1843.
The sentence "May 'it' haunt their houses pleasantly, and no one wish to lay 'it'" contains two so-called "reference errors." However, there is no ambiguity and nobody would claim Charles Dickens is a poor writer.


One of these implicit rules is that nouns in adverbial phrases count for the most recent noun, and nouns in adjective phrases--mere attendants to and parts of other nouns--do not count. A reference to a noun in an adjective phrase can be forced through context, generally with some degree of awkwardness, but when no context forcing is present, the default is to ignore the adjective phrase for the purpose of reference but not ignore the adverb phrase.

For example, from the same story (do a search to find the place):
"But I am sure I have always thought of Christmas time, when it has come round"
The "it" refers to "Christmas time," even though "Christmas time" is inside an adverb phrase.
 
  • #38
I took the following example of a reference error from this web site:

http://www.webster.edu/acadaffairs/asp/wc/grammar.html

I hate my brother. This is a problem.

The site prefers: "I hate my brother. My hatred is a problem."

I have two complaints.

1. No matter how poor the grammar, there is no ambiguity in it. When she says "This is a problem" only the most obtuse would be confused about what she means.

2. No one ever talks or writes like what the site prefers. You don't need to be Charles Dickens to write better than that.

If the grammar in my puzzle is incorrect, it is none the less common usage. And it is far more ambiguous than BicycleTree gives it credit for. The unwritten 'it' could very well mean the 86th floor or it could mean the Empire State Building, or even the elevator. That ambiguity is at the very heart of the puzzle. You can't put your elbow in your ear, but I can. My elbow is flexible and your ear is not too high.
 
Last edited by a moderator:
  • #39
My case is, your problem statement is grammatically correct (in spite of what some grammarians say about reference errors), and not ambiguous for reasons I said. If you can find an example of a sentence by a respected and fairly conventional author where you have
1. verb
2. object
3. adverb phrase for the verb
4. noun in the adverb phrase
5. adjective phrase containing a noun and modifying the noun in the adverb phrase
6. pronoun immediately following that is intended to modify either the object or the noun in the adjective phrase instead of the noun in the adverb phrase
7. yet what the pronoun modifies is not forced (one example of forcing would be "he" or "she" which is forced to modify a masculine or feminine noun; basically it's forced if assuming the modified object is other than the intended one causes the sentence to make no sense)

then I'll grant your point. But I don't believe such a sentence exists. Intuitively, in such a situation, the pronoun would always modify the noun in the adverb phrase.
 
  • #40
about the linguistic, do the same rule apply for other language.
 
  • #41
Okay, I see BT's point, but I still don't buy the rule that a pronoun must refer to the most recently used noun that fits it. You can read almost anything by anyone and see the rule broken again and again. It just isn't worth following, IMO. In fact, the rule is stated in jimmy's link:
"A PRONOUN must always have an ANTECEDENT; it must refer back to the most recently used noun."
She breaks the rule even while stating it! antecedent is the most recently used noun, but it clearly refers to pronoun.
And given poetic license, I don't think looking to fiction writers as a model of grammatical correctness is a good idea either. But here's a sentence that I made up:

Bob takes a gift to his dog in the hospital and unwraps it.

Does that work?
 
  • #42
ArielGenesis said:
about the linguistic, do the same rule apply for other language.
Sure, some languages have similar rules and some don't. But linguists study more than just grammar. I haven't built up a collection of linguistics links yet, but http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Linguistics look like a decent overview.
 
  • #43
"A PRONOUN must always have an ANTECEDENT; it must refer back to the most recently used noun."
In this sentence, there is another rule in use. Because of the semicolon, the second part of sentence is a restatement or clarification of the first part of the sentence, and is therefore expected to follow similar structure. So the "it" subject of the second part of the sentence can be expected to correspond to the "pronoun" subject of the first part of the sentence. The rules for determining what pronouns should refer to what nouns are complex, but they exist. For example, if the sentence were constructed this way:
"A PRONOUN must always have an ANTECEDENT; if it is referred back to, the reference must be by the following pronoun."
Then it would break the rule of semicolon similar-structure, and plainly the revised sentence is awkward and more difficult to read because of this (you expect, reading it, that "it" will mean "pronoun," and it doesn't).

"Bob takes a gift to his dog in the hospital and unwraps it" is an example of the forcing I mentioned. The sentence makes no sense unless you interpret the "it" to refer to the gift. However, I can tell you I had to do a double take when reading the sentence; the only natural intepretation until you get to the forcing is to expect that any pronoun coming will refer to "dog," and it's a surprise (and awkward) when you come to the forcing and have to re-evaluate the sentence.
 
  • #44
Wow, it's amazing we ever manage to understand each other. :-p This is getting more interesting, but I don't think I know enough to continue. Just one more thing- it seems a pronoun doesn't always need to have an antecedent:

It is amazing that we manage to understand each other.

Okay, just one more thing:

When I put the book on the shelf, it falls.

on the shelf is (functionally) an adverb phrase? So you're saying it should refer to shelf? I would normally take that sentence to be ambiguous. Heh, now I'm having too much fun- you must agree the following is ambiguous?

When I put the book on the shelf on the wall, it falls.
 
Back
Top