What Makes the Six Colors in Newton's Experiment Special?

  • B
  • Thread starter JB321
  • Start date
In summary: You can't have infinite shades of one color, but you can have a great many shades of different colors.
  • #1
JB321
6
2
I recently read an article written by Newton which outlined the process and results of his "crucial experiment".
From what I understand, Newton says that light can continue to be split until it reaches the basic colors and then it simply stops. What is it about these frequencies of light that makes them so special? Are there six exact frequencies that are the base of all the colors? Or are there ranges of frequencies that Newton simplified to one color? I asked my physics teacher and he responded with, "I don't know..."

Thanks in advance,
JB
 
  • Like
Likes berkeman
Physics news on Phys.org
  • #2
Is that "6 colors" part mentioned by Newton?
 
  • #3
JB321 said:
Or are there ranges of frequencies that Newton simplified to one color?

That's the likely explanation. If he used a prism, then he saw bands of color each of which contain a range of wavelengths.

I should mention, however, that the color of each wavelength can be matched by weighting of three suitable light sources. This gives rise to the CIE standard observer color matching functions which are the basis of the CIE L*a*b* color system.
 
Last edited:
  • #4
There are more than 6 colors. Far more. But the real idea to take away from this is that a perfect spectrum is continuous and is measured in either wavelength or frequency, not colors. You can take a slice of the spectrum and expand it until you reach the resolution limit of your system, the spectrum doesn't simply stop at some point.

Remember that color is subjective. My father is red-green colorblind, so his experience of color is very different from mine, and there are supposedly people with more than 3 types of cone cells in their eyes (the cells that allow you to see color), so their experience of color is also far different from mine.

On the other hand, the frequency/wavelength of light is not subjective. Everyone with the proper measurement device will agree on the frequency and wavelength of any sample of light.
 
  • Like
Likes 1oldman2, merricksdad, ProfuselyQuarky and 1 other person
  • #5
So why is it that if you split a ray of light several times, you will end up with an irreducible color? Say you isolate the blue section of a spectrum emitted from a prism. Why does the blue frequency not separate into different frequencies of blue? (maybe it does, but Newton seemed to say that it didn't) Is is because the frequencies are so close together that it is difficult to isolate them?
 
  • #6
JB321 said:
So why is it that if you split a ray of light several times, you will end up with an irreducible color?
Because the solution of the temporal part of EM wave equation is of sinusoidal form ##\sin \omega t## (or equivalently ##\cos \omega t##) which has a definite frequency/wavelength called harmonics. Any EM radiation of arbitrary temporal profile can be decomposed into sum of these harmonics. That's why when a light ray is dispersed by prism or diffraction grating you get light rays each with a single indivisible frequency.
 
  • #7
JB321 said:
Why does the blue frequency not separate into different frequencies of blue? (maybe it does, but Newton seemed to say that it didn't) Is is because the frequencies are so close together that it is difficult to isolate them?

Yes, any spectrometer will have a finite resolution.
 
  • #8
Drakkith said:
and there are supposedly people with more than 3 types of cone cells in their eyes (the cells that allow you to see color), so their experience of color is also far different from mine.
Tetrachromats have a fourth cone that is slightly different than the usual green ones we all have. They can detect finer gradients of green than the rest of us. They might see subtleties that we don't. But that's about it.
 
  • #9
blue_leaf77 said:
Because the solution of the temporal part of EM wave equation is of sinusoidal form ##\sin \omega t## (or equivalently ##\cos \omega t##) which has a definite frequency/wavelength called harmonics. Any EM radiation of arbitrary temporal profile can be decomposed into sum of these harmonics. That's why when a light ray is dispersed by prism or diffraction grating you get light rays each with a single indivisible frequency.
But, in fact, you do not.
 
  • Like
Likes CWatters
  • #10
jbriggs444 said:
But, in fact, you do not.
Do not what?
 
  • #11
blue_leaf77 said:
Do not what?
You do not get a set of discrete indivisible rays.
 
  • Like
Likes CWatters
  • #12
There are infinite shades of blue.
 
  • Like
Likes OmCheeto and CWatters
  • #13
jbriggs444 said:
You do not get a set of discrete indivisible rays.
Whether it is discrete or continuous after being dispersed depends on the spectrum of the light. Some vapour lamps have discrete spectrum, neglecting the effect of line broadening.
 
  • #14
Khashishi said:
There are infinite shades of blue.
How can you have infinite shades of one colour? The blue light, for instance, has only a given range of wavelengths 450 - 494 nm so that range is not infinite.
 
  • #15
Simon Peach said:
How can you have infinite shades of one colour? The blue light, for instance, has only a given range of wavelengths 450 - 494 nm so that range is not infinite.

450.001, 450.002, 450.003...or however fine you want to make it.

Are you only thinking in terms of integers?
 
  • #16
pixel said:
450.001, 450.002, 450.003...or however fine you want to make it.

Are you only thinking in terms of integers?
Yes I agree that the numbers could be infinite, but the colours can't be. But thus is only nit picking really
 
  • #17
Simon Peach said:
Yes I agree that the numbers could be infinite, but the colours can't be. But thus is only nit picking really

I think Khashishi was just making the point that you can divide the portion of the spectrum corresponding to blue light into an infinite number of pieces.
 
  • Like
Likes Fervent Freyja
  • #18
Simon Peach said:
Yes I agree that the numbers could be infinite, but the colours can't be. But thus is only nit picking really

You are correct when talking about human perception of color as it takes a certain amount of color difference to be just noticeable. Instrumentally, we can resolve spectral differences finer than that.
 
  • Like
Likes sophiecentaur
  • #19
Simon Peach said:
How can you have infinite shades of one colour? The blue light, for instance, has only a given range of wavelengths 450 - 494 nm so that range is not infinite.
How many possible frequencies are there in this range?
 
  • #20
PeroK said:
How many possible frequencies are there in this range?
Just as there are infinite points between 0 and 1, so there are infinite frequencies between 450 and 494.
 
  • #21
PeroK said:
How many possible frequencies are there in this range?
The number of 'identifiably' different frequencies depends entirely on the bandwidth / signal to noise ratio, of your measurement system. The eye is not all that good as a measuring instrument. Only under stringent conditions do you actually need the 'millions of colours' that high quality colour displays use. Those 'millions' refer to the number of points in two dimensional CIE colour space and not just spectral component frequencies and implies something like 0.1% discrimination over the spectral range. As a bandwidth for measurement of frequency, that is pretty rubbishy. Frequency measurement can be done easily to 10 significant figures in some ranges of EM waves.
We specify colours in our language with much cruder accuracy. This link has a picture of the Cie Chromaticity chart, showing a very crude categorisation of areas with named colours. We can distinguish between large areas of colour (patches / contours are just detectable) when the colour space is divided into around a million smaller areas but we don't 'remember' colours with anything like that level of discrimination. (You can't take a new tie home from the shop and be sure that it will match the shirt that you left in your wardrobe but the difference will scream at you when you lay them side by side).
 
  • #22
And how about those fifty shades of grey, eh?
 
  • Like
Likes sophiecentaur
  • #23
"Color" can have totally different and even conflicting meanings if you don't define what you are measuring.

If you mix light from a red laser and a green laser, and adjust them to the correct brightnesses, you could produce light that a human would see as identical to a yellow laser. But a prisim would reveal that this light had nothing in common with an actual yellow laser. The idea that red plus green makes yellow is pure biology, not physics.
 
  • Like
Likes phans, FactChecker and merricksdad
  • #24
JB321 said:
I recently read an article written by Newton which outlined the process and results of his "crucial experiment".
From what I understand, Newton says that light can continue to be split until it reaches the basic colors and then it simply stops. What is it about these frequencies of light that makes them so special? Are there six exact frequencies that are the base of all the colors? Or are there ranges of frequencies that Newton simplified to one color? I asked my physics teacher and he responded with, "I don't know..."

Thanks in advance,
JB

From what I remember, Newton posited 7 not 6 colors. He chose 7 mostly for numerological reasons, i.e. purely because 7 is a holy number in Christianity.
 
  • Like
Likes DaveC426913
  • #25
The first thing that must be understood is that "color" is a psycho-physical phenomenon, and its *perception* by humans is dependent on 2 systems: the electromagnetic wave/photon (i.e., light) receptors of the retina, and the processing of the output from those receptors by the brain. Normally, humans have these receptors (called "cones" by physiologists) that are tuned to have a maximum transfer function (i.e., how much output gets generated per input) at 3 specific wavelengths of light, that are recognized as being "red", "green" & "blue". Photons of light have a specific wavelength associated with them (i.e., in whatever relativistic reference frame an observer is in), and each photon of a specific frequency generates a specific output for each of these 3 tunings of receptors, and thus, there is a 3-D vector space for the receptors' output. The brain processes this triple coordinate as being a total "color". If the coordinates all the have same value, the brain processes it as "white" of some type (including "grey"); if the coordinates are very high in only one of those coordinates, the brain processes it as the respective color (including a darker shade).

Unless one is looking at the output from a laser device, the light that any set of receptors observe is in the form of many individual wavelengths that can be approximated as a continuous spectrum. This spectrum is in essence the "true color" of any light, with the cones & brain mapping that out to some "perceived color". To a certain level of accuracy, there are infinitely many combinations of individual wavelengths, each at some intensity, that is perceived by any particular brain as the same exact color; this is the reason why television works - a real spectrum is observed by an camera, which then produces a 3-D vector space signal that can be displayed by a display device, which a viewer would perceive in 3-D vector space as being the same as what would be perceived if viewing that original spectrum. As one might expect, this 3-D signal is best matched to human color perception by matching up with the wavelengths that correspond to the cones' maximum transfer functions, and is the reason why color is regarded as being a RGB (red-green-blue) coordinate.

There is a class of spectra called pure color (maybe it is called something else) in which the spectrum is only a single wavelength; such spectra is perceived by the brain as having a certain 3-D color coordinate value, and typically a display cannot reproduce such a spectra (except for those spectra that exactly match the output spectra of the individual display). However, these pure colors basically map to a 1-D curve within the 3-D vector space (or 2-D if intensity is normalized); there are plenty of other colors that the human color perception perceives, but these are artificial colors. A lot of these colors are close to a true color, but there is one particular section of color that is totally artificial - the colors from purple to red, which is due to the fact that the mixture of pure colors must be between these 2 colors, but yet not be along the pure color spectrum. A prism separates out light because the index of refraction is slightly different for different wavelengths; the glorious natural phenomenon of a rainbow has the same mechanism, although the particulars of geometric optics that makes it so is quite an interesting topic in its own right.

Now, as for the OP's original question of there being "6 colors", typically being in order along the pure color spectrum as violet, blue, green, yellow, orange, red, that is just perception as the brain has a hard time picking out any more of a fine gradation of colors. Now this might be my opinion, but when I look at a rainbow, I tend to notice a very large section between blue & green, that is typically know as cyan, so I would say that there really are 7 colors, not 6.

Hope this helps.
 
  • #26
swampwiz said:
when I look at a rainbow, I tend to notice a very large section between blue & green, that is typically know as cyan, so I would say that there really are 7 colors, not 6.
Actually, a rainbow is not a good source for observing spectral colours because the background sky desaturates them grossly, even for the most striking rainbow. If you look at a well spread out spectrum, you will be very aware of the gradation from one wavelength colour to the next and that there are distance and identifiable colours between any two of the 'well known' ones. It's just that we don't bother to use any more than seven colours to describe the spectrum and it isn't worth making a finer scale. That is, unless you are interested getting colorimetric fidelity and good matching of copied colours. In which case you find hundreds of different and distinguishable by A-B comparison (but not memorable) colours along the spectrum.
 
  • #27
DaveC426913 said:
Just as there are infinite points between 0 and 1, so there are infinite frequencies between 450 and 494.
But are do the difference between the infinitesimal frequencies make a substantial difference ?
 
  • #28
In video and digital art, an alternative to the Red, Green, Blue color space is HSB, or Hue, Saturation, Brightness. For both HSB and RGB, digital systems today are assumed to need at least 8 bits per channel (24 bits total) to produce photographic quality images.

So, ignoring saturation and brightness, those systems devote 8 bits to defining just the hue. That means 256 colors circling the rainbow from red, through yellow, green, blue, and back to red. (Excluding white, black, pink, brown ext. ).

Early digital systems with less then 8 bits per channel had to inject noise into the signal or other tricks to avoid obvious banding or problems due to insufficient numbers of colors.

Thus we can reasonably presume that typical humans can perceive around 200-300 colors, assuming high brightness and saturation, and not counting such variations as colors.
 
  • #29
TheLegendOfCars101 said:
But are do the difference between the infinitesimal frequencies make a substantial difference ?

that's irrelevant ... the point is there IS a difference
 
  • #30
TheLegendOfCars101 said:
But are do the difference between the infinitesimal frequencies make a substantial difference ?
"substantial" is a poorly defined term. It depends on what you are trying to detect and use color for. The eye may be very limited, but a prism could be used to detect very small frequency differences.
 
  • #31
"So why is it that if you split a ray of light several times, you will end up with an irreducible color? Say you isolate the blue section of a spectrum emitted from a prism. Why does the blue frequency not separate into different frequencies of blue?"

The three types of cones in the human eye are bandpass filters that respond to broad, overlapping ranges of colors. The "green cone", for example, doesn't just detect green wavelengths, it responds to a wide range of colors from orange to yellow to green to aqua. The "red cone" also responds to yellow and orange, as well as red wavelengths, but in different proportions than the green cone. When both red and green cones respond with equal intensity (and there is little blue cone response) the brain interprets this combination as yellow.

The important point to understand is that an individual cone cannot distinguish among the different colors it responds to. For example, a green cones cannot tell the difference between blue-green and yellow-orange colors, and responds in exactly the same way to both. Likewise, a blue cone would respond in exactly the same way to two shades of blue that were close to the same wavelength. You would only be able to distinguish between them if one of the blue colors stimulated the green cone more than the other.
 
  • #32
Lish Lash said:
"So why is it that if you split a ray of light several times, you will end up with an irreducible color? Say you isolate the blue section of a spectrum emitted from a prism. Why does the blue frequency not separate into different frequencies of blue?"

The three types of cones in the human eye are bandpass filters that respond to broad, overlapping ranges of colors. The "green cone", for example, doesn't just detect green wavelengths, it responds to a wide range of colors from orange to yellow to green to aqua. The "red cone" also responds to yellow and orange, as well as red wavelengths, but in different proportions than the green cone. When both red and green cones respond with equal intensity (and there is little blue cone response) the brain interprets this combination as yellow.

The important point to understand is that an individual cone cannot distinguish among the different colors it responds to. For example, a green cones cannot tell the difference between blue-green and yellow-orange colors, and responds in exactly the same way to both. Likewise, a blue cone would respond in exactly the same way to two shades of blue that were close to the same wavelength. You would only be able to distinguish between them if one of the blue colors stimulated the green cone more than the other.

You have to draw a huge distinction between colour vision and spectroscopy. Human colour vision is very approximate and uses just three wide and sensors to analyse and classify approximately, the whole of the visible spectrum of light entering the eye. It treats single spectral lines and light with complicated spectra in exactly the same way. The brain uses just three signals to categorise all the light from an object. That sensation is what we call colour. A spectrometer, otoh, splits the light into regions of FINITE bandwidth. It's the same as a radio receiver or RF spectrum analyser. The notion of a 'single' frequency is a bit nonsensical in this context. It's just a bit of convenient Maths. Every measurement method has a Resolution Bandwidth that blurs the resulting analysis. Zero bandwidth means no energy admitted, meaning you couldn't measure it. The resolution of a "prism" is limited by the width of the slit. Too thin a slit would mean not enough light gets through. Catch 22.
 
  • #33
The initial question was about colours. As I said before there cannot be infinite colours, the range of the visible electromagnet spectrum is "Visible light is usually defined as having wavelengths in the range of 400–700 nanometres (nm), or 4.00 × 10−7 to 7.00 × 10−7 m, between the infrared (with longer wavelengths) and the ultraviolet (with shorter wavelengths)". So unless you're going to split a nanometre there cannot be infinite range of colours.
 
  • #34
Simon Peach said:
The initial question was about colours. As I said before there cannot be infinite colours, the range of the visible electromagnet spectrum is "Visible light is usually defined as having wavelengths in the range of 400–700 nanometres (nm), or 4.00 × 10−7 to 7.00 × 10−7 m, between the infrared (with longer wavelengths) and the ultraviolet (with shorter wavelengths)". So unless you're going to split a nanometre there cannot be infinite range of colours.
What's the problem with splitting a nanometre?
 
  • #35
Simon Peach said:
So unless you're going to split a nanometre there cannot be infinite range of colours.

You can easily split a spectrum up into sub-nanometer wavelength. This is routinely done in spectroscopy.
 

Similar threads

Replies
7
Views
1K
  • Classical Physics
Replies
21
Views
1K
  • Programming and Computer Science
Replies
1
Views
585
  • Engineering and Comp Sci Homework Help
Replies
4
Views
908
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
Replies
4
Views
1K
  • Special and General Relativity
Replies
25
Views
941
  • Other Physics Topics
Replies
2
Views
2K
Replies
7
Views
4K
  • Special and General Relativity
Replies
21
Views
707
Back
Top