Questions About PhDs: What's the Process and How Long Does it Take?

  • Programs
  • Thread starter RestlessMind
  • Start date
  • Tags
    Curiosity
In summary: I demonstrated, took classes, did research, and taught. I was lucky if I got a day off every other week. I also moonlighted as a radio DJ, helped out in a friend's lab, and wrote for the newspaper.Sometimes, I miss those days. It all depends on the person. Some people have no problem with the work load. Others crack under the pressure.
  • #71
Has graduate school become more intense than it was several decades ago? One thing's for sure: PhDs are taking longer and longer to complete. But thanks to technology and the Internet, we have all these new time-saving devices (Herbert Gintis said that it takes far less time to get papers than it was several decades ago). But since we now have a much larger knowledge base, it takes longer for us to absorb it all, so these time-saving devices only partially compensate for all the extra knowledge we have to take in.

And I see all these grad students and professors who have families, and I wonder - how much time do they really spend on their families? For people like me who will probably never get a family, won't we have some extra spare time just for ourselves?

===

I guess it's sort of funny, but I'm seeing some of the discussed effects right now. Almost every working hour of my day is now spent on studying, Wikipedia, research papers, and various science forums. I don't even have the patience or attention span for computer games anymore. I can load up Team Fortress 2, get fragged several times, then give up, lose patience, and return to reading planetary science papers. Which are far more interesting, than, let's say, the new changes that get introduced with each new game (be it Total War or FPS). It's really quite funny.

But there are so many amazing developments that are coming out of other fields (biology in particular) too, and I'd like to be able to keep up with them too. And of course, if I read those things too (which I do), the result is that I don't even know if there is anything beyond science, or the tools used to do science.
 
Last edited:
Physics news on Phys.org
  • #72
Simfish said:
Has graduate school become more intense than it was several decades ago? One thing's for sure: PhDs are taking longer and longer to complete. But thanks to technology and the Internet, we have all these new time-saving devices (Herbert Gintis said that it takes far less time to get papers than it was several decades ago). But since we now have a much larger knowledge base, it takes longer for us to absorb it all, so these time-saving devices only partially compensate for all the extra knowledge we have to take in.

Grad school, in itself, has probably become more intense. But, that's only a symptom of the underlying problem (which you correctly identify)- it takes longer to learn enough to be able to make original contributions to science.

The most straightforward proof of this is the average age of a research scientist obtaining their first major grant- for the NIH, it's the R01 award (or it's equivalent):

http://www.google.com/url?sa=t&sour...sg=AFQjCNFmaRwKfB1SdjHpliwGu7pE4sv28Q&cad=rja

The age has steadily increased over time, from 40 in 1998 to 42 in 2000 (and leveling off between 42.5 and 43 since then)- by contrast, in 1970, it was 34.

This has far-reaching implications, because the majority of research-intensive positions are so-called "soft money" positions- 100% of a person's salary derives from grant dollars. Tenure-track positions, by contrast, are subsidized by the institution.

So, the increasing age of "independence" means that a person spends longer and longer in a post-doctoral or other temporary position- this means a person lives longer and longer with uncertainty about their future career prospects, the stakes get higher and higher (because people have a limited 'shelf life'), and the research environment *in general* becomes more and more stressful.

That's why it's so critical to realize that a PhD is not an endpoint. It's merely the first milestone on the path to becoming a scientist.
 
  • #73
G01 said:
I've met a few post-docs from Germany in my time in grad school so far.

They are some of the most extremely independent scientists I've met. I say this as a grad student who is NOT a totally independent scientist yet. (I bug my adviser and the older grad students in my lab quite regularly.)

We have focused mostly on whether the immersion of grad school is overkill, but perhaps we should focus more on it's purpose.

There is something that happens to the way one thinks when you immerse yourself like one does in grad school. If you spend 60+ hours a week thinking about one topic, your brain becomes built to think about that thing. Your thinking process changes, permanently.

I'm noticing this happening to me now. I see the results in the older grads in my lab and in my adviser. For example, my adviser, the 6th year student, and the post-doc in my lab are all trained to think about science, all the time. They end up coming up with ideas that I find brilliant, to say the least. They are small things, like neat ways to align optics, and big things, like novel measurements we could run and then publish. They are all ideas that I would never have thought of, at my current stage of education anyway. Slowly, I notice myself having neat, new ideas the more I immerse myself in the field.

Everyone else in my lab has already thought of most of my ideas :rolleyes:, but still, the point is that this immersion trains you to be a successful scientist. Without these novel ideas, my adviser wouldn't get grants, the grad students wouldn't publish, and science would not get done. And my personal experience tells me that this 60+ hour/week immersion is how you teach your brain to start thinking in this essential way.

So, before we go onto a tangent asking if the grad school work load is some strange, cultic, hazing ritual, let's ask if maybe there is method to the madness. Maybe there is a reason the system exists the way it does? Perhaps the intense immersion has an educational effect? I wonder what the other grad students and PhDs in this thread think about this point?

I think that's definitely worth talking about. I see your point. I don't know if that really is the case, but your logic is sound. If it takes a large amount of time for the way someone thinks to change in order to learn a field (and I do think it takes a long time), then how much more must a person change to be able to make actual contributions to the field, and how much time must this take?

I'm not sure that alone explains the 60 hours per week, but to me it certainly is reason enough to need a graduate school education and experience.

If it actually takes 60 hours/week for 6 years to think like a scientist (I'm not convinced, but we can talk about it!) then that's just what it takes. However, what I took away from your description of your more experienced colleagues is that what made them scientists is that they were creative and original. I don't see how 60 hrs/wk for 6 yrs on a single thing makes someone more creative, though. Perhaps it's just experience and knowledge. In which case all that time would help.

Einstein once said that imagination is more important than knowledge. If he was right, then I wonder how spending all your time from the moment you step into grad school until...well, forever, (since apparently it only gets 'worse' after grad school) on physics alone is helpful in that regard.


Andy Resnick said:
Grad school, in itself, has probably become more intense. But, that's only a symptom of the underlying problem (which you correctly identify)- it takes longer to learn enough to be able to make original contributions to science.

The most straightforward proof of this is the average age of a research scientist obtaining their first major grant- for the NIH, it's the R01 award (or it's equivalent):

http://www.google.com/url?sa=t&sour...sg=AFQjCNFmaRwKfB1SdjHpliwGu7pE4sv28Q&cad=rja

The age has steadily increased over time, from 40 in 1998 to 42 in 2000 (and leveling off between 42.5 and 43 since then)- by contrast, in 1970, it was 34.

This has far-reaching implications, because the majority of research-intensive positions are so-called "soft money" positions- 100% of a person's salary derives from grant dollars. Tenure-track positions, by contrast, are subsidized by the institution.

So, the increasing age of "independence" means that a person spends longer and longer in a post-doctoral or other temporary position- this means a person lives longer and longer with uncertainty about their future career prospects, the stakes get higher and higher (because people have a limited 'shelf life'), and the research environment *in general* becomes more and more stressful.

That's why it's so critical to realize that a PhD is not an endpoint. It's merely the first milestone on the path to becoming a scientist.

The "shelf life" you speak of is the most worrying thing I've read on this topic. What exactly do you mean by that? I'm 23 with 2 more years of undergraduate ahead of me. I'd like to be able to support a family once I hit 30, but I would have the uncertainties and low pay of graduate school, post docs, and a mysterious "shelf life."

I'm perfectly willing to work in industry, but so many of you make it sound like I could not be a physicist (or even an engineer!) if I did so. The examples I hear of are "financial, programming, analyst." I would be miserable. :/

Not to hijack, but if someone could address those concerns I would be most grateful.
 
  • #74
Mobusaki said:
The "shelf life" you speak of is the most worrying thing I've read on this topic. What exactly do you mean by that? I'm 23 with 2 more years of undergraduate ahead of me. I'd like to be able to support a family once I hit 30, but I would have the uncertainties and low pay of graduate school, post docs, and a mysterious "shelf life."

That's a fair question- what I say is based on my personal experience (including witnessing what goes on around me).

The first step in becoming a productive researcher is training- the PhD (and post-doc) trained me to do more than just design, carry out and analyze an experiment- it trained me to ask a question worth answering.

Postdocs are little more than hired guns- they come into apply what they already know to a new problem. Doing research means progressing beyond what you already know, and pushing into the unknown.

What I have seen is that people who do not push themselves into new territory, people who simply stay and re-hash the same techniques/questions over and over again, stop being productive. Over time, science progresses beyond what they are doing, and they get left behind- quite literally.

Why would someone continue to do the same thing, over and over again? Lots of reasons. A common one is that they develop a reputation for doing something new and sexy, and so they just keep on doing that same thing. Another is that they find it easier/safer to get a steady paycheck by becoming a glorified technician in a large, well-funded lab.

That's "shelf life". What's your shelf life? That's up to you.
 
  • #75
Diracula said:
Even if string theory turns out to be incorrect, he's a physicist by most people's definition of physics and has put out an extremely large body of work that is considered important by a large group of physicists.

1) Most outside of physics have a very skewed definition of physics

2) The stuff that Witten has done is may be useful to a few people in quantum gravity, but very few physicists are working in that area, and his work doesn't have any obvious application to anything outside of that extremely narrow field.

If relativity or QM turn out to be 'incorrect' in 100 years because someone finds a better theory it doesn't suddenly make Einstein and Dirac spectacularly unsuccessful at physics.

Because relativity and QM are close enough to how the universe behaves so that you can do useful things with them. String theory has had absolutely no connection with observable reality that anyone has been able to come up with, and it's an example of a "spectacular unsuccessful" theory.

Also Einstein was a spectacularly unsuccessful physicist once he ended up at IAS.

No, it's not like that at all. Different people work at different rates. If you've worked with anyone else you know this. Everyone runs into unexpected and time consuming difficulties at work, doing a PhD, whatever. Not everyone fixes those difficulties at the exact same rate. This is slightly different then making it rain if you want it to.

And for Ph.D.'s the type of problem that you run into is pretty independent of the person that gets the problem.
 
  • #76
Diracula said:
I mean, they keep stats on this stuff. 2% of physics PhDs complete their degree in 3 years or less.

And it's invariably either because they completed some of the Ph.D. work before they started the program, or they are using the English system which is different than the US. In the UK, the clock starts counting only after you do the coursework.

And if you want to argue everyone works on physics at the exact same rate and efficiency, I'm really not sure what to say.

I'm saying that at the Ph.D. level, individual efficiency and productivity doesn't matter because how long it will take depending on the magnitude of the setback which is pretty much totally out of your control.

Witten is a good example of this. It doesn't matter how brilliant or efficient he is, if he is one the wrong path because he is unlucky, he is on the wrong path.
 
  • #77
Mobusaki said:
I think that's definitely worth talking about. I see your point. I don't know if that really is the case, but your logic is sound. If it takes a large amount of time for the way someone thinks to change in order to learn a field (and I do think it takes a long time), then how much more must a person change to be able to make actual contributions to the field, and how much time must this take?

I'm not sure that alone explains the 60 hours per week, but to me it certainly is reason enough to need a graduate school education and experience.

If it actually takes 60 hours/week for 6 years to think like a scientist (I'm not convinced, but we can talk about it!) then that's just what it takes. However, what I took away from your description of your more experienced colleagues is that what made them scientists is that they were creative and original. I don't see how 60 hrs/wk for 6 yrs on a single thing makes someone more creative, though. Perhaps it's just experience and knowledge. In which case all that time would help.

Einstein once said that imagination is more important than knowledge. If he was right, then I wonder how spending all your time from the moment you step into grad school until...well, forever, (since apparently it only gets 'worse' after grad school) on physics alone is helpful in that regard.

I agree that creativity is important, but there is other stuff involved. You hit the nail right on the head in this post. Experience and knowledge are just as important as creativity. You need all three. In a sense it's like a painter without paint and an easel. They could be the most creative painter in the world, but they aren't going to paint anything if they don't have the necessary supplies.
 
  • #78
Mobusaki said:
I do think that when you've spent so much time in academia you develop an elitist attitude. It is really hard to not look down on people who know less than you about a thing sometimes.
You don't have to. It is enough to remember that you know extremely little about all the other subjects where trained people know everything about. You need not look down on people just because they are not interested to the same degree as you in the same things as you and hence know much less about it than you.
 
  • #79
G01 said:
Slowly, I notice myself having neat, new ideas the more I immerse myself in the field.

Everyone else in my lab has already thought of most of my ideas :rolleyes:, but still, the point is that this immersion trains you to be a successful scientist. Without these novel ideas, my adviser wouldn't get grants, the grad students wouldn't publish, and science would not get done. And my personal experience tells me that this 60+ hour/week immersion is how you teach your brain to start thinking in this essential way.

So, before we go onto a tangent asking if the grad school work load is some strange, cultic, hazing ritual, let's ask if maybe there is method to the madness. Maybe there is a reason the system exists the way it does? Perhaps the intense immersion has an educational effect?

''expertise is a characteristic of individuals and is a consequence of the human capacity for extensive adaptation to physical and social environments. Many accounts of the development of expertise emphasize that it comes about through long periods of deliberate practice. In many domains of expertise estimates of 10 years experience or 10,000 hours deliberate practice are common.'' (http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Expert )
 
  • #80
Okay, so this thread has been going on for 5 pages now and is essentially people who aren't even grad students yet clutching their pearls over imaginary scenarios. Look, if you have the goods and think you are a genius who can complete a PhD in 4 years while working less than 40 hours a week, fine! Go do it then, I'm sure no one will fault you for being too smart but at least stop hemming and hawing over a system you don't even have experience in, armchair grad students. There is a REASON grad school takes as long as it does and if you choose to believe that it's because all the grad students are lazy, inefficient, and stupid then that's your shortsightedness.
 
  • #81
MissSilvy said:
Okay, so this thread has been going on for 5 pages now and is essentially people who aren't even grad students yet clutching their pearls over imaginary scenarios. Look, if you have the goods and think you are a genius who can complete a PhD in 4 years while working less than 40 hours a week, fine! Go do it then, I'm sure no one will fault you for being too smart but at least stop hemming and hawing over a system you don't even have experience in, armchair grad students. There is a REASON grad school takes as long as it does and if you choose to believe that it's because all the grad students are lazy, inefficient, and stupid then that's your shortsightedness.

I think you've greatly misunderstood the intent of the people posting in this thread.
 
  • #82
Andy Resnick said:
Grad school, in itself, has probably become more intense. But, that's only a symptom of the underlying problem (which you correctly identify)- it takes longer to learn enough to be able to make original contributions to science.

The most straightforward proof of this is the average age of a research scientist obtaining their first major grant- for the NIH, it's the R01 award (or it's equivalent)...

The age has steadily increased over time, from 40 in 1998 to 42 in 2000 (and leveling off between 42.5 and 43 since then)- by contrast, in 1970, it was 34.

I actually think the rising age of R01 awards has more to do with the funding and job crunch that started in the 70s than an actual increase in the time it takes to become a productive scientist. I've known plenty of extremely independent, successful postdocs who were essentially idea-generators for their PI.
 
  • #83
I actually think the rising age of R01 awards has more to do with the funding and job crunch that started in the 70s than an actual increase in the time it takes to become a productive scientist. I've known plenty of extremely independent, successful postdocs who were essentially idea-generators for their PI.

Wow, could you care to elaborate more about this? Do the postdocs suggest a huge number of ideas, and leave the professor to choose which one is the best? And why does the professor use the postdocs as idea generators? Does generating ideas have to take that much time?
 
  • #84
ParticleGrl said:
I actually think the rising age of R01 awards has more to do with the funding and job crunch that started in the 70s than an actual increase in the time it takes to become a productive scientist. I've known plenty of extremely independent, successful postdocs who were essentially idea-generators for their PI.

Not exactly- the increased competition has resulted in an ever decreasing percentage of successful proposals.

http://publications.nigms.nih.gov/loop/images/20051031_th2.jpg

This particular institute had a funding line of around 37% from 2000-2003 (meaning the top 37% of proposals received funding), and then that dropped quite precipitously to 25% in 2005. Current paylines NIH-wide are around 15%.

It's difficult to make sense of that number by itself, since in addition to the increased number of proposals, NIH has increased the number of funded proposals- plus, people could re-submit their proposal a second time (until recently a third time as well).

New investigators have consistently gotten a 5-10% bump in their scores just by being "new investigators"- NIH has done this specifically to encourage new investigators.

So there's two trends- 1) decreasing success rates overall, and 2) a longer period of time before a *first* successful proposal.

Finally, I'd argue that any post-doc who serves as an 'idea generator' for a PI is being taken advantage of. The post-doc gets nothing and loses everything by giving someone else a fundable idea.
 
  • #85
Finally, I'd argue that any post-doc who serves as an 'idea generator' for a PI is being taken advantage of. The post-doc gets nothing and loses everything by giving someone else a fundable idea.

The thing with generating ideas, though, is that scientists are oftentimes so resistant to new ideas that they think that people with new ideas are crazy - to the point that people with crazy ideas are often dismissed as crackpots (you can see more if you read articles about a lot of famous scientists - see Irene Pepperberg, Max Tegmark, Geoff Marcy, etc). If you're working under an established scientist, however, then the ideas are more likely to be given credence.
 
  • #86
Simfish said:
The thing with generating ideas, though, is that scientists are oftentimes so resistant to new ideas that they think that people with new ideas are crazy - to the point that people with crazy ideas are often dismissed as crackpots...

This is entirely not true. Scientists have to be open to new, crazy ideas, else they wouldn't get grants or publish!

There is a huge difference between a scientist with a crazy idea and a crackpot with a crazy idea.

A crack pot is someone who proposes a crazy idea, with little to no evidence supporting the idea. The idea is analyzed by other scientists, and determined to not be true based on theoretical and experimental evidence. Yet, the crackpot still supports the idea and dismisses any valid arguments against it as "political bias" or something of the like. THAT is a crackpot.

A scientist can have a crazy idea, and go with it. This happens regularly. For instance, I would consider the metamaterial cloak designed by David Smith's group at Duke ( http://www.sciencemag.org/content/314/5801/977.short ) to be a crazy idea, wouldn't you? Yet, it turned out to be backed up by sound theoretical and experimental logic, and they decided to run with it.

Instead if an idea turns out to NOT be supported by any good evidence, the scientist knows when to accept this and move on or modify the idea. This is the crucial difference between this scientist and a crackpot.
 
Last edited:
  • #87
This is entirely not true. Scientists have to be open to new, crazy ideas, else they wouldn't get grants or publish!

There is a huge difference between a scientist with a crazy idea and a crackpot with a crazy idea.

A crack pot is someone who proposes a crazy idea, with little to no evidence supporting the idea. The idea is analyzed by other scientists, and determined to not be true based on theoretical and experimental evidence. Yet, the crackpot still supports the idea and dismisses any valid arguments against it as "political bias" or something of the like. THAT is a crackpot.

A scientist can have a crazy idea, and go with it. This happens regularly. For instance, I would consider the metamaterial cloak designed by David Smith's group at Duke ( http://www.sciencemag.org/content/314/5801/977.short ) to be a crazy idea, wouldn't you? Yet, it turned out to be backed up by sound theoretical and experimental logic, and they decided to run with it.

Instead if an idea turns out to NOT be supported by any good evidence, the scientist knows when to accept this and move on or modify the idea. This is the crucial difference between this scientist and a crackpot.

The thing is, though, that scientists often tend to be so over-suspicious to crackpots that the suspicion often takes legitimate theories as bycatch too.

Look at this, for example http://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=96897162:

Or at least that's what I argued in my grant proposal. Apparently, the review panel was not impressed. On August 19, just two weeks after the "by George he's got it" moment, I received a letter from the panel that essentially asked me what I was smoking. They implied I was crazy to even imagine that a bird brain could master the language and cognitive skills I was hoping to demonstrate. And they further implied I was even crazier to shun the accepted approach of operant conditioning and adopt this highly suspect method of social interaction.

And this http://www.wired.com/wiredscience/2011/02/geoff-marcy-qa/#more-49005:

Geoff Marcy: You want the real answer? It’s personal. After I got my PhD at Santa Cruz, I was really lucky and I got a post-doctoral fellowship at the Carnegie Institute of Washington, which is in Pasadena.

And in brief, my research wasn’t going very well. A Harvard astronomer criticized my PhD thesis. And I felt pretty bad. Everyone seemed smarter than me. I felt a little bit like an impostor, like they haven’t figured out that I’m not as smart as them, that I’m not really smart enough to be a scientist. I thought okay, well now the jig is up. Maybe my career is over.

But I still have a year and a half on my post-doc! I remember one morning in my apartment in Pasadena, as I took my shower, thinking, I can’t suffer like this anymore. I’ve got to just enjoy myself, do research that really means something to me.

So I thought, what do I care about? I would love to know if there were other planets around other stars.This was a question that nobody was asking. It was 1983, and nobody was even talking about planets. Even our own solar system was considered boring at the time.
So by the time I turned off the shower, I knew how I was going to end my career. I quickly realized that this was kind of a lucky moment. By knowing that I was a failure, I was free. I could just satisfy myself, and hunt for planets — even though it was a ridiculous thing to do. At that time, I hadn’t heard of anybody actively hunting for planets.

Wired.com: How did people react when you started doing it?

Marcy: They were embarrassed for me. I might as well be looking for little green men, or how the aliens built the pyramids in Egypt, or telekinesis. Even professional astronomers at that time associated planets around other stars with science fiction.

It’s very hard to imagine that now. Look at this meeting. We have probably 500 talks and posters on extrasolar planets. It’s hard to put yourself in a mindset in which planets were considered the lunatic fringe. But that’s what it was.
 
  • #88
Simfish said:
The thing is, though, that scientists often tend to be so over-suspicious to crackpots that the suspicion often takes legitimate theories as bycatch too.

First of all, these are outlier examples that make for good news stories. I don't think they can be used to judge the current state of science as a whole. I'm also not a psychologist, so I won't comment on the science involved in the first example.

Either way,
Legitimate crazy ideas should be subject to high suspicion and scrutiny. The fact that they made it through the scrutiny is what makes them legitimate crazy ideas.

The crazier the idea, the harder the scrutiny.
 
  • #89
Okay sure. Given that, then maybe it might be advantageous for postdocs to work as "idea generators" under PIs and not as "idea generators" on their own? Given that "idea generators" are probably going to generate a lot of ideas that will get tagged as crazy?
 
  • #90
I don't think there really are that many ideas that get tagged as crazy. I'm pretty sure most of those coming from academics/post-docs/PhD students are actually sane, but just, well, you know, good.
 
  • #91
Simfish said:
The thing is, though, that scientists often tend to be so over-suspicious to crackpots that the suspicion often takes legitimate theories as bycatch too.

Peer review is not a perfect system, but it's a lot better than the alternatives. Everyone has a story about how their (rejected) paper/proposal got an unfair review.

Even so, the stories you mentioned also serve as excellent examples, per G01, on the difference between scientists having a new idea and crackpots- the scientists, both of them, after getting rejected, *worked their a$$es off* getting proof of their ideas. They didn't sit around and moan about how some cabal is out to get them- they put in long days and years slowly building up *evidence* to support their ideas.
 
  • #92
Andy Resnick said:
Peer review is not a perfect system, but it's a lot better than the alternatives. Everyone has a story about how their (rejected) paper/proposal got an unfair review.

Even so, the stories you mentioned also serve as excellent examples, per G01, on the difference between scientists having a new idea and crackpots- the scientists, both of them, after getting rejected, *worked their a$$es off* getting proof of their ideas. They didn't sit around and moan about how some cabal is out to get them- they put in long days and years slowly building up *evidence* to support their ideas.

You make a good point. My counter point would be to ask why anyone who worked their *** off 60 hours per week for 4-6 years to join a scientific community would be ostracized by that same community for having an outrageous idea? I'm not saying we should all smoke dope and hold hands, but I also don't think we should be so quick to ostracize people who worked just as hard as everyone else to be able to voice their ideas.

I understand (and hope) that it happens rarely, but I do think there is a strong tendency to spit people out of the community who disagree with you.

Look at the scientists who disagree with global warming. An argument can be made the scientific community has tried to silence their voice. Let them disagree. So what if they're wrong? They'll offer their evidence, global warming supporters will offer theirs, and then people can decide themselves what to believe.

I think that's how it should be in all cases, instead of shutting people down because we're convinced their wrong. It surprises me how, with all the twists and turns in human knowledge up to this point, we can still be so sure in what we know so as to try to destroy others based on what they think they know.
 
Last edited:
  • #93
I always marvel at the number of undergraduates who are certain that "the system is broken" without having even taken a close look at it. Here we have someone a couple days into their research program, and that's enough to tell.

Yes, a lot of unconventional ideas don't get through peer review. A lot of excellent ideas don't get through peer review. For those of us who have reviewed for the NSF, proposals are ranked in three categories - "must fund", "fund if possible" and "do not fund". At the last panel I was on the tentative "must fund" category was between 2 and 2.5x the entire budget.

Welcome to life in the big city.

I can remember the last three unconventional proposals I reviewed. Two of them had really clever ideas. Neither one had done the legwork to demonstrate that the proposal would actually address these ideas, and they both got turned down. The third one was maybe a little less clever, but the PI had outlined a plan that was well thought through, and would provide a definite answer one way or the other. He ended up at the top of our list of proposals to fund.
 
  • #94
Vanadium 50 said:
I always marvel at the number of undergraduates who are certain that "the system is broken" without having even taken a close look at it. Here we have someone a couple days into their research program, and that's enough to tell.

Yes, a lot of unconventional ideas don't get through peer review. A lot of excellent ideas don't get through peer review. For those of us who have reviewed for the NSF, proposals are ranked in three categories - "must fund", "fund if possible" and "do not fund". At the last panel I was on the tentative "must fund" category was between 2 and 2.5x the entire budget.

Welcome to life in the big city.

I can remember the last three unconventional proposals I reviewed. Two of them had really clever ideas. Neither one had done the legwork to demonstrate that the proposal would actually address these ideas, and they both got turned down. The third one was maybe a little less clever, but the PI had outlined a plan that was well thought through, and would provide a definite answer one way or the other. He ended up at the top of our list of proposals to fund.

I hope you aren't referring to me. :( I'm not convinced the system is broken *or* working, just that some healthy debate about it can only make it better.
 
  • #95
Andy Resnick said:
So there's two trends- 1) decreasing success rates overall, and 2) a longer period of time before a *first* successful proposal.

Both are indicative of a the funding crunch I was suggesting. My opinion is that the first trend is fueling the second. After all- the best indicator of future success is past success, which might create some bias towards established researchers.

Finally, I'd argue that any post-doc who serves as an 'idea generator' for a PI is being taken advantage of. The post-doc gets nothing and loses everything by giving someone else a fundable idea.

::shrug:: Having a big name as lead on a proposal might boost its chance of acceptance. I've never really dealt with NIH, as my field wasn't bio/health related, but I've seen it help on other proposals.

Peer review is not a perfect system, but it's a lot better than the alternatives. Everyone has a story about how their (rejected) paper/proposal got an unfair review.

My worry is that as funding and jobs get more competitive, the peer review system will start to break down as the incentives grow to torpedo other's work. Scientists are only human.
 
Last edited:
  • #96
Simfish said:
The thing with generating ideas, though, is that scientists are oftentimes so resistant to new ideas that they think that people with new ideas are crazy

I've found that to be rarely true.

If you're working under an established scientist, however, then the ideas are more likely to be given credence.

That also may not be true. There are some idea that come from established scientists (Roger Penrose's ideas on neuroscience) that are bad crackpot.
 
  • #97
Simfish said:
The thing is, though, that scientists often tend to be so over-suspicious to crackpots that the suspicion often takes legitimate theories as bycatch too.

He didn't have particular difficulty getting funding.

Also grant committees are a lot like admission committees, you sometimes get lucky and hit someone that likes the way that you approach problems, and you sometimes don't.

One problem with grant committees is that there just ain't enough money. In the NSF grant committees I've been on, there's usually one in ten proposals that wouldn't get funded if given infinite amounts of money.
 
  • #98
Mobusaki said:
You make a good point. My counter point would be to ask why anyone who worked their *** off 60 hours per week for 4-6 years to join a scientific community would be ostracized by that same community for having an outrageous idea?

Because 1) you aren't ostracized for having outrageous ideas and 2) most outrageous ideas turn out to be bad ones. You need people that tell you what the flaws are with your ideas so that you can figure out whether to go ahead with them.

Also you need a pool of crazy ideas. The thing that separates someone who is productive from someone who isn't, is that the productive scientist figures out that crazy idea #324 doesn't work so they stop working on that and works on crazy idea #534.

I also don't think we should be so quick to ostracize people who worked just as hard as everyone else to be able to voice their ideas.

There's a difference between criticism and ostracism. The fact that your best friend is willing to spend three hours going into point by point why crazy idea #324 just won't work tells you that they are your best friend.

Also "hard work" doesn't mean much. You can work hard and be wrong.

I understand (and hope) that it happens rarely, but I do think there is a strong tendency to spit people out of the community who disagree with you.

Physicists love to argue. If I go to someone with my latest new idea and they tell me how brilliant it is, then I'm disappointed. The reason you have collaborators is so that they can tell you want a bad idea you just came up with is. Then if you still think is a good idea, you hit back, and after punching each other for an hour, you all shake hands and get something to eat.

I think that's how it should be in all cases, instead of shutting people down because we're convinced their wrong.

If you've going through a physics Ph.D. program, then merely having someone tells you that you are wrong won't shut you up.

It surprises me how, with all the twists and turns in human knowledge up to this point, we can still be so sure in what we know so as to try to destroy others based on what they think they know.

That's the whole culture of science. You get into a room of people that try to destroy your ideas.
 
  • #99
ParticleGrl said:
Both are indicative of a the funding crunch I was suggesting.

Except there isn't a funding crunch- NIH's budget *doubled* in the 2000s and dollars allocated to research in general (including NSF, NOAA, NASA, DOE, Navy, AFRL, national labs, etc) has held fairly steady at 2.5% of GDP for the past 40-50 years:

http://www.cbo.gov/ftpdocs/82xx/doc8221/06-18-Research.pdf

"From 1953 to 2004, real (inflation-adjusted) spending for R&D in the United States rose at an average annual rate of 4.7 percent, faster than the 3.3 percent average growth of GDP".

You may have some correct elements in your idea, but "funding crunch" is not one of them.
 
  • #100
The problem with a lot of PHD material is that it's not that good. It tends to be boring, tedious minor regiments to larger ideas.

The outcome of attaining a PHD is to produce original work, but there's no guaranteeing this work will be interesting or useful. It doens't take a PHD to produce interesting, original research.
 
  • #101
elfboy said:
The problem with a lot of PHD material is that it's not that good.

And you are in a position to judge the worth and usefulness of Ph.D. dissertations across a multitude of different fields, are you?

It tends to be boring, tedious minor regiments to larger ideas.

Believe it or not, but science progresses because of these boring, tedious, minor regiments. Forget what the popular science books tell you about scientific revolutions and paradigm shifts and all that. In reality, science advances slowly through these small contributions from many, many individuals.
 
Last edited:
  • #102
Interesting article I just found:
http://sciencecareers.sciencemag.org/career_magazine/previous_issues/articles/2011_03_04/caredit.a1100020
 
  • #103
Simfish said:
Interesting article I just found:
http://sciencecareers.sciencemag.org/career_magazine/previous_issues/articles/2011_03_04/caredit.a1100020

I can't make heads or tails out of the story:

1. Canovas was a post-doc for 4 years and then ran out of funding. Yet, he thinks he was fired for other reasons? Most post docs are 2 year appointments, so a 4 year appointment seems reasonable. Given the information in the article, I don't understand why he thinks he was laid off for dubious reasons. Am I missing something or has the article left out important information??

2. His adviser gave him publishing rights, the journal asked him to add his adviser as an author, he ruined his relationship with his adviser by accusing is adviser of firing him for dubious reasons, adviser doesn't consent, so journal doesn't publish. It seems straightforward. His adviser has every right not to consent to be an author. I wouldn't think it was censorship, either. Revenge- possibly, but it's not illegal to refuse to be an author on a paper.


It seems he cut away friendly ties with his adviser when his funding ran out. Possibly he had good reasons for this, I don't know. Either way, Canovas shouldn't expect his adviser to help him out after he brought a law suit against the guy! Canovas' issue seems to be with making and keeping friends. Connections are important. No one is going to argue that.
 
Last edited:
  • #104
G01 said:
And you are in a position to judge the worth and usefulness of Ph.D. dissertations across a multitude of different fields, are you?
I'm sorry G01, but I believe he is. After all, he is the Dean of Troll University, so he should know what he's talking about.
 
  • #105
It seems odd to me that people would put themselves through at least 4 years of constant hard work (right after 4 years of undergrad!) if the phd work was boring. Hopefully the majority of people doing it find the work they are doing interesting and satisfying.

lol. I'll file that in the "things to not worry about" cabinet. It needs more stuff in it! :P
 

Similar threads

Replies
3
Views
903
  • STEM Academic Advising
Replies
4
Views
610
  • STEM Academic Advising
Replies
5
Views
849
  • STEM Academic Advising
Replies
9
Views
2K
  • STEM Academic Advising
Replies
3
Views
829
Replies
7
Views
442
  • STEM Career Guidance
2
Replies
35
Views
8K
  • STEM Academic Advising
Replies
11
Views
1K
  • STEM Academic Advising
Replies
5
Views
1K
  • STEM Academic Advising
2
Replies
45
Views
4K
Back
Top