What causes the green penumbra in this picture?

In summary, the conversation discussed the effects of different light sources on a black bottle cap, leading to the discovery of a green shadow in the presence of a white light source. The participants speculated on the reason for this phenomenon, considering factors such as the material's color reflecting property and the different wavelengths in white light. They also discussed the potential impact of camera filters and the limitations of human perception in detecting color differences. Ultimately, the conversation highlighted the complexity of light and how it can affect our visual perception.
  • #1
Adel Makram
635
15
I exposed a black bottle cap by two light sources ( LEDs), one source is white and the other one is red as in the picture.
I noticed that one penumbra toward the red light source is reddish in color which makes sense because this area is only exposed to the red source. What makes a puzzle is the other penumbra toward the white source is green.
What is the reason? No green light source was in the room and the screen is a white paper.
 

Attachments

  • IMAG0207.jpg
    IMAG0207.jpg
    18.8 KB · Views: 663
Physics news on Phys.org
  • #2
I think this is a product of the materials color reflecting property, white light contains different wave lengths, something that is black usually absorbs light. This material is "glossy" so it reflects some light. My best guess is that the material is reflecting that particular wave length while absorbing the others.

http://www.physicsclassroom.com/Class/light/U12L2c.cfm
 
  • #3
I took the image and chose a white balance point in the "green" part.
I
IMAG0207balanced.jpg
 
  • Like
Likes Adel Makram
  • #4
gjonesy said:
I think this is a product of the materials color reflecting property, white light contains different wave lengths, something that is black usually absorbs light. This material is "glossy" so it reflects some light. My best guess is that the material is reflecting that particular wave length while absorbing the others.

http://www.physicsclassroom.com/Class/light/U12L2c.cfm
I tried with other material and I got the same effect.
 
  • #5
I looked at the first photo and appears to be a green cast off from the brown mug also. I've noticed that LEDS tend to have a different effect then regular incandescent white light bulbs. The green penumbra may just be the product of shadow. Most white LEDS have a blue-ish tint. So this just may be the light source itself.

I copied this from Wikipedia , white LEDS are...

"The existence of blue LEDs and high-efficiency LEDs quickly led to the development of the first white LED, which employed a Y3Al 5O
12:Ce, or "YAG", phosphor coating to mix down-converted yellow light with blue to produce light that appears white."

Primary colors yellow and blue make...green?
 
Last edited:
  • Like
Likes Adel Makram
  • #6
gjonesy said:
I looked at the first photo and appears to be a green cast off from the brown mug also. I've noticed that LEDS tend to have a different effect then regular incandescent white light bulbs. The green penumbra may just be the product of shadow. Most white LEDS have a blue-ish tint. So this just may be the light source itself.
If the green shadow is due to the light source, the exposed white paper with the white source only would yield a green hue too.
I exposed a blank white paper with the white LED and I used some apps to roughly measure the color of the area exposed to the white LED and I compared it with the color of the area which appears green in the first experiment. ( see the picture).
I could not find any green hues anywhere in the paper which is only exposed to the white LED. While the small shadow which appears green in the first experiment gets green color using that apps.
This means the white LED is not the reason for that green color.
 

Attachments

  • comparison.jpg
    comparison.jpg
    25.1 KB · Views: 520
  • #7
This is not uncommon and it's often only when looking at a photograph that it becomes really obvious. Your eyes tend to compensate when looking at the real thing. Two different light sources, sunlight incadescent/ mfl / led / flash will throw their own shadows. The shadow will be largely filled in by other sources but, if the other source has a different spectrum, the shadow will appear coloured. The unshadowed part reflects a mix of the two lights (appearing to be the correct 'white' colour) whilst the shadow only reflect the light from one. The camera may automatically adjust the colour balance to bring the average of the scene to its chosen 'white' and any of the shadows will appear to be slightly different from the 'white'. Anything that's not the chosen white will appear to have a colour tint.
Photographers have to try very hard to avoid the effects of mixed illumination. Filling in interior shots with filament lamps can have some really weird effects that people in the room just won't be aware of.
 
  • Like
Likes Adel Makram and PietKuip
  • #8
sophiecentaur said:
This is not uncommon and it's often only when looking at a photograph that it becomes really obvious. Your eyes tend to compensate when looking at the real thing. Two different light sources, sunlight incadescent/ mfl / led / flash will throw their own shadows. The shadow will be largely filled in by other sources but, if the other source has a different spectrum, the shadow will appear coloured. The unshadowed part reflects a mix of the two lights (appearing to be the correct 'white' colour) whilst the shadow only reflect the light from one. The camera may automatically adjust the colour balance to bring the average of the scene to its chosen 'white' and any of the shadows will appear to be slightly different from the 'white'. Anything that's not the chosen white will appear to have a colour tint.
Photographers have to try very hard to avoid the effects of mixed illumination. Filling in interior shots with filament lamps can have some really weird effects that people in the room just won't be aware of.
Thank you for showing me how the camera can modify the colors of the scene.
But I even noticed that by my naked eyes before using the camera. I thought of a retinal compensation, but this may work in a narrow interface between two areas (something similar to Mach effect) unlike my observation that the whole shadow was green. I am not happy with both explanations.
 
  • Like
Likes gjonesy
  • #9
I wasn't referring to transitions between areas. I was referring to the various 'whites' that are visible in your picture and others. There are many optical illusions in which a spot colour will appear different, in the same picture, when presented against two different local backgrounds. The eye does its best and it tends to be a lot better than the auto feature on a camera. It uses context and past knowledge to get the best information it can from a scene.
But the shadows in your picture are actually illuminated with different light so they will, of course, look different. That difference may show up more on a simple photograph than when viewing the scene by eye. If the background 'should' look the same, in the brain's estimation, then you will probably won't notice the differences.
 
  • #10
gjonesy said:
Primary colors yellow and blue make...green?
That statement relates to subtractive mixing.
We are dealing with Additive Colour Mixing in this thread, afaics. The three 'primaries' that are used in additive mixing are Red Green Blue (Hence RGB TV signals). The sort of colour mixing we all learn about first is subtractive, with paints, which absorb wavelengths selectively and the reflected colour is due to different wavelengths being subtracted from the incident white. A yellow pigment can be achieved in many ways but it will usually reflect some reddish light, along with some greenish light. A blue pigment will reflect mostly short wavelengths and absorb reds and yellows but let through some of the green looking wavelengths. So the mixture will look green. The three most useful primary pigments are Cyan, Yellow and Magenta ('minus red', 'minus blue' and 'minus green') and the spectra of the reflected light tends to be broad band; the three spectra will overlap a lot and combinations of these primaries can produce the gamut of visible colours. There is a problem with trying to get all colours with subtractive mixing with just three pigments and that is the resulting colours can look very dark when you want a good saturated colour.
 
  • Like
Likes gjonesy
  • #11
sophiecentaur said:
That statement relates to subtractive mixing.
We are dealing with Additive Colour Mixing in this thread, afaics. The three 'primaries' that are used in additive mixing are Red Green Blue (Hence RGB TV signals). The sort of colour mixing we all learn about first is subtractive, with paints, which absorb wavelengths selectively and the reflected colour is due to different wavelengths being subtracted from the incident white. A yellow pigment can be achieved in many ways but it will usually reflect some reddish light, along with some greenish light. A blue pigment will reflect mostly short wavelengths and absorb reds and yellows but let through some of the green looking wavelengths. So the mixture will look green. The three most useful primary pigments are Cyan, Yellow and Magenta ('minus red', 'minus blue' and 'minus green') and the spectra of the reflected light tends to be broad band; the three spectra will overlap a lot and combinations of these primaries can produce the gamut of visible colours. There is a problem with trying to get all colours with subtractive mixing with just three pigments and that is the resulting colours can look very dark when you want a good saturated colour.

I know the quote yellow and blue make green is true with paint. With light I'm not so sure. My best guess is the type of light used plays a role in combination with the semi shaded area.

http://www.bing.com/images/search?q...ac22e840fa9fef47025811252bfdd95fo0&ajaxhist=0

It seems reflected white light from LEDS produce a blue/green type cast off depending on the angle of reflection. reference above link
 
  • #12
sophiecentaur said:
But the shadows in your picture are actually illuminated with different light so they will, of course, look different. That difference may show up more on a simple photograph than when viewing the scene by eye. If the background 'should' look the same, in the brain's estimation, then you will probably won't notice the differences.
So what do you expect if I use green and white LEDs instead of red and white LEDs? Will the shadow that appeared green in the previous example be green too?
 
Last edited:
  • #13
gjonesy said:
I know the quote yellow and blue make green is true with paint. With light I'm not so sure.
The best way to predict what you get with additive colour mixing is by reference to the CIE chromaticy chart. It describes the human 'colour space'. Any colour you care to think of is at a point on that chart. See this link and many others. If you want to predict what an additive mixture of colours will look like, you draw a line between the points you chose and the perceived colour will lie at a point along the line. The position depends upon the relative weightings and, for equal weights, the colour will be at the mid point. If you take a point in the yellow region and a point in the blue region, it will be greenISH but, as it lies very near the centre (white), it will be a very de-saturated green or even white with a hint of green. With RG and B primaries at the vertices of a triangle, you can produce any colour within the sides of the triangle. Good TV systems use the best primaries available at the time, which are as bright as they can be made and as near the curved (spectral) edge of the chart as possible.
 
  • Like
Likes gjonesy
  • #14
Adel Makram said:
So what do you expect if I use green and white LEDs instead of red and white LEDs? Will the shadow that appeared green in the previous example be green too?
In a region of shadow with zero contribution from one of the light sources, you will just see the other one reflected. There is no reason to expect anything other than that - unless the 'context' in the image suggest to your brain that the Red + White contributions are, in fact the 'correct 'white illuminant', in which case the shadow will look (very desaturated) green. Likewise for the green and blue LEDs, the shadow may be interpreted as (very desaturated) red. A camera or spectrometer will tell you the 'truth' of the situation, which may not be what you actually 'see'.
There is really no such thing as White. It can lie anywhere near the centre of that CIE Chromaticity diagram.. What you see will depend on a combination of the illuminant and the reflecting surface. The White that's used in TV displays can vary, according to the taste of the viewer, the characteristics of the display and the camera. There are agreed standards, which programme producers use.

The colour of light from any source can easily be far from isotropic; that's no surprise. It will depend on the quality of the source. You would hope that the relative intensities would'nt vary too much around the axis. Many times it won't matter though.
 
  • #15
sophiecentaur said:
In a region of shadow with zero contribution from one of the light sources, you will just see the other one reflected.
That is exactly what I observed when I used a green and white LEDs ( see the picture).
sophiecentaur said:
There is no reason to expect anything other than that - unless the 'context' in the image suggest to your brain that the Red + White contributions are, in fact the 'correct 'white illuminant', in which case the shadow will look (very desaturated) green. Likewise for the green and blue LEDs, the shadow may be interpreted as (very desaturated) red. A camera or spectrometer will tell you the 'truth' of the situation, which may not be what you actually 'see'.
The shadow toward the white LED (on the right side of the picture) appears red as intuitively expected.
This is clear by both naked eyes and by the camera.
If the brain interprets the mix of the green and white laying on the screen as white, then why the shadow appears red? Are you saying that the brain has a similar compensation like modern cameras that adjust the color and saturation of hues according to the average colors of the scene? If so, what is the name of this phenomena? I am a doctor but I did not recall that I have studied such thing before. I only know Mach effect which appears at edges between areas of different illuminations because of retina compensation.
Why do we have to consider this as optical illusion? If I have to study the spectrum of the light in this red area, will not I find the light comes toward the red spectrum?
 

Attachments

  • IMAG0239.jpg
    IMAG0239.jpg
    16.3 KB · Views: 503
Last edited:
  • #16
Adel Makram said:
Are you saying that the brain has a similar compensation like modern cameras that adjust the color and saturation of hues according to the average colors of the scene?
Yes - but our perception is pretty complex, of course. In a limited area, we decide on what is 'white' and anything else is interpreted as 'colour' - albeit very desaturated. However, there are some scenes in which one area with given RGB values and which appears 'coloured' and yet another area, elsewher, with the same RGB values can be see as white, and the background in the other area can appear to be coloured.
You don't have to look at colours to get this sort of effect. There are many monochrome illusions in which you interpret areas with the same surface brightness as, in one place, pale grey and in other places, as dark grey. See this link.
The poor old brain is doing its best to interpret what the eyes are telling it but can often get it wrong.
 
  • Like
Likes Adel Makram
  • #17
Adel Makram said:
will not I find the light comes toward the red spectrum?
That's one way of saying it but all three of the 'whites' that are seen - (the main area and the two penumbra) would all be considered to be white because the source LEDs are used as a white illuminant. What we perceive is not 'an illusion' but we can be presented with deliberate illusions that can fool us. As with many of our abilities, it's when they actually fail to work that we can begin to find out how they operate.
Let's face it, when you go outside in the winter, you would call the light 'white' and when you go indoors into a brightly lit room, you would also call the lighting 'white'. Likewise in a kitchen or office with cheapo flu tubes. None of those 'whites' are the true white because there is not one. Some are bluish, orangeish, greenish and, after five minutes with one illumination, you are quite happy to assess and match the colour of some clothing or paint. Within limits, you can usually get it right, too.
We needed to evolve with the ability to compensate for a variety of white illuminants but, during our evolution, we were never presented with LEDs, CFLs or mercury arcs so we never evolved to cope with them.
 
  • Like
Likes Adel Makram
  • #18
If the illumination of the background of the white screen by a mix of white and green is interpreted as "white" and other shadows as colored, then this effect should disappear when this background screen is hidden. Like when I cute another piece of black paper and fit it on the white screen but leaving an area for the shadow then see what its color.
 
  • #19
Adel Makram said:
If the illumination of the background of the white screen by a mix of white and green is interpreted as "white" and other shadows as colored, then this effect should disappear when this background screen is hidden. Like when I cute another piece of black paper and fit it on the white screen but leaving an area for the shadow then see what its color.
Do you conduct the experiment in total darkness, except for the visible bit of the screen, lit by just the LED?
Whatever you 'see' is psychological and the result of the actual spectrum of the light reaching your eye. What 'colour' is the patch of light to you? If it changes with and without the other light source present then some of that light is getting to the patch of screen. Nothing in this sort of experiment can actually alter the wavelengths of the light so it just has to be down to perception, comparison and memory. There are loads of papers about colour vision. The basic tristimulus theory that's applied to Colour TV just has to be pretty credible because 'everyone' who sees colour TV pictures, these days, agrees that they are pretty damn good.
Land's Retinex theory is interesting and you can find about it in this link, http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.475.6201&rep=rep1&type=pdf and many others.
 
Last edited by a moderator:
  • #20
sophiecentaur said:
Do you conduct the experiment in total darkness, except for the visible bit of the screen, lit by just the LED?
Yes. It was in total darkness.
 
  • #21
Adel Makram said:
Yes. It was in total darkness.
OK, so you had one source of light, shining on a white /neutral screen. How would you be seeing anything other than the effect of that light source? Anything you saw was interpreted by your brain as a very desaturated colour. I would suggest that, if you woke up in the night and that's all you could see, you'd probably have assessed it as white.
Is there anything more to it than that? (i.e. any other experimental detail)
I wonder, for instance, just how non reflective your black card was. It is actually very hard to get a 'good' black with paint or printing.
 
  • Like
Likes Adel Makram
  • #22
sophiecentaur said:
If you take a point in the yellow region and a point in the blue region, it will be greenISH but, as it lies very near the centre (white), it will be a very de-saturated green or even white with a hint of green

just a question of curiosity for sophiecentaur, myself and a friend were looking at light photos, trying to figure this out. it seems I can see very slight color differences in reflected LED light. I'm not color blind, but I see a different (just few colors) spectrum. What causes this?

http://www.bing.com/images/search?q...ac22e840fa9fef47025811252bfdd95fo0&ajaxhist=0

for instance in the image link I see "white blue gray olive green, and brown" co-worker can only see "white blue and green"
 
  • #23
gjonesy said:
just a question of curiosity for sophiecentaur, myself and a friend were looking at light photos, trying to figure this out. it seems I can see very slight color differences in reflected LED light. I'm not color blind, but I see a different (just few colors) spectrum. What causes this?

http://www.bing.com/images/search?q...ac22e840fa9fef47025811252bfdd95fo0&ajaxhist=0

for instance in the image link I see "white blue gray olive green, and brown" co-worker can only see "white blue and green"
I looked at the image (the one with the LED with light shining from it?) and all I can say is that it doesn't really constitute a valid experiment. You say that you are not colour blind but have you tried the Ishihara colour vision test? You could google it but the test is based on a printed version (the original) afaiaa and so it would not be valid to assess yourself on the TV displayed image. Colour perception is an individual thing and the Ishihara test gives a range of results, covering 'normal' colour vision. I guess it is possible that you and your friend just happen to be slightly apart from each other in this respect. Your friend may be more adventurous in his / her description of colours?
Try to get hold of the book of colour vision tests; your local Uni library will probably have a copy.
 
  • #24
Adel Makram said:
I exposed a black bottle cap by two light sources ( LEDs), one source is white and the other one is red as in the picture.
I noticed that one penumbra toward the red light source is reddish in color which makes sense because this area is only exposed to the red source. What makes a puzzle is the other penumbra toward the white source is green.
What is the reason? No green light source was in the room and the screen is a white paper.
QA It's not green, it's cyan the opposite of red.
Adel Makram said:
I exposed a black bottle cap by two light sources ( LEDs), one source is white and the other one is red as in the picture.
I noticed that one penumbra toward the red light source is reddish in color which makes sense because this area is only exposed to the red source. What makes a puzzle is the other penumbra toward the white source is green.
What is the reason? No green light source was in the room and the screen is a white paper.
It's cyan not green, cyan is the opposite of red. Magenta is the opposite of green. Shadows tend to take on an opposite hue.
 
Last edited:
  • Like
Likes sophiecentaur

What causes the green penumbra in this picture?

The green penumbra in this picture is caused by a phenomenon known as diffraction. This occurs when light waves pass through a small opening or around an object, causing them to bend and interfere with each other, creating areas of light and dark.

Is the green penumbra a result of reflection or refraction?

The green penumbra in this picture is a result of neither reflection nor refraction. It is a result of diffraction, which is the bending of light waves as they pass through a small opening or around an object.

Why does the green penumbra appear only on one side of the object?

The green penumbra appears on only one side of the object because it is caused by the bending of light waves as they pass through a small opening or around an object. In this picture, the light source is coming from one direction, causing the diffraction to occur on one side of the object.

Can the size of the green penumbra be changed?

Yes, the size of the green penumbra can be changed by altering the size of the opening or object through which the light waves pass. A smaller opening or object will result in a larger penumbra, while a larger opening or object will result in a smaller penumbra.

Does the color of the object affect the color of the green penumbra?

No, the color of the object does not affect the color of the green penumbra. The color of the penumbra is determined by the wavelength of the light waves and the interference patterns created by diffraction.

Similar threads

  • Other Physics Topics
Replies
7
Views
3K
  • Other Physics Topics
Replies
11
Views
422
Replies
9
Views
862
  • Classical Physics
Replies
21
Views
933
  • General Discussion
Replies
9
Views
952
Replies
14
Views
2K
  • Other Physics Topics
Replies
6
Views
17K
  • Computing and Technology
Replies
3
Views
362
Replies
7
Views
1K
Back
Top