A ball struck by a cue in billiards with English goes straight at first....

AI Thread Summary
When a cue ball is struck on its side with English, it initially travels straight due to the way the force is applied and the high friction between the cue tip and the ball. Unlike ball-to-ball collisions, where the balls are hard and elastic, the cue-ball interaction allows for a longer contact time, enabling the cue to effectively 'throw' the ball in the direction of the cue. The friction during this contact causes the ball to slide before it begins to roll, which influences its trajectory. The discussion highlights that the cue's softness and the friction coefficient play significant roles in determining the ball's movement. Ultimately, while the cue ball can be influenced to go straight, it cannot be made to turn towards the English during the collision itself.
  • #151
sophiecentaur said:
In the video I am referring to (Post #24) the ball path is parallel with the cue.
Nope. Watch the video again.

sophiecentaur said:
That was what the question needed an explanation for, as far as I can see.
Nope. Read the OP again. It asks about the difference to ball-on-ball, where the initially resting ball goes off aprox. along the contact normal. That difference is ~15° in that video.
 
Physics news on Phys.org
  • #152
A.T. said:
Nope. Watch the video again.Nope. Read the OP again. It asks about the difference to ball-on-ball, where the initially resting ball goes off aprox. along the contact normal. That difference is ~15° in that video.
This is in the top post and is what I have been addressing.
poolplayer said:
I heard that it is because the ball and cue can be thought as unity so the force only propagates to the cue direction.
There have been a lot of statements and you have obviously been looking at others; the thread is almost endless.
 
Last edited:
  • #153
poolplayer said:
Anyway, I am going to post probably decisive evidence against the friction between the ball and table. I hang a ball with a rope in the air and stroke the ball with right English.


Thanks for the experiment. A very good idea - the vertical string doesn't apply horizontal forces to the ball, so it's all cue contact force.

poolplayer said:
So, now we can focus on the real force that makes the ball goes rightwards from the normal direction.
Well it's obviously friction, and the required friction coefficient is below 0.3, so entirely feasible
 
  • #154
A.T. said:
Well it's obviously friction, and the required friction coefficient is below 0.3, so entirely feasible
Glad to hear that. Here, 0.3 comes from tan(15°) because the tangential force is [tan(θ) * normal force] , right? Some say that friction coefficient between the ball and cue tip is around 0.6, so it would be much greater than 0.3.

But, how can I know how much of the tangential force affects the movement of the ball? I thought I could use energy conservation to know speed of the ball to tangential direction, but it seems not good because I don't know how much energy I applied to the ball...
 
  • #155
poolplayer said:
Anyway, I am going to post probably decisive evidence against the friction between the ball and table. I hang a ball with a rope in the air and stroke the ball with right English.
That's the best evidence so far and it suggests that I am wrong in some respects. (Great to see the experiment is continuing, btw)
I am definitely more interested in getting a right answer to this than 'not being proved wrong'.
 
  • Like
Likes poolplayer
  • #156
poolplayer said:
Glad to hear that. Here, 0.3 comes from tan(15°) because the tangential force is [tan(θ) * normal force] , right?
Yes.

poolplayer said:
Some say that friction coefficient between the ball and cue tip is around 0.6, so it would be much greater than 0.3.
The static friction coefficient is a limit. The actual ratio can be anything less than that.

poolplayer said:
But, how can I know how much of the tangential force affects the movement of the ball?
Not sure what you mean. All of it affects the movement via F = ma.
 
  • #157
A.T. said:
Not sure what you mean. All of it affects the movement via F = ma.
Maybe I should not say that the tangential force induces torque... but the tangential force somehow relates to the torque, right? When I think of a ball on a conveyor belt, I guess that the ball rolls if its moment of inertia is small and the ball moves with the belt without rolling if its inertia is large. Is it correct?

What I want to do is to estimate the tangential force if possible and confirm that it is near tanθ of the normal force.
 
  • #158
poolplayer said:
Maybe I should not say that the tangential force induces torque... but the tangential force somehow relates to the torque, right? When I think of a ball on a conveyor belt, I guess that the ball rolls if its moment of inertia is small and the ball moves with the belt without rolling if its inertia is large. Is it correct?
To confirm that linear and angular velocity changes are consistent with the assumption of a single impulse from the cue, check if the following relation holds:

w/v = 5/2 * r/R2

or:

w/v = 5/2 * sin(θ)/R

where:

w : angular speed
v : speed
r : lever arm of the force around the center
R : radius of the sphere
θ : angle between contact normal and the ball velocity / net force.

poolplayer said:
What I want to do is to estimate the tangential force if possible and confirm that it is near tanθ of the normal force.
It is per definition, if the above assumption holds.
 
  • #159
poolplayer said:
What I want to do is to estimate the tangential force if possible and confirm that it is near tanθ of the normal force.
To get the direction out of it, you only need to know the ratio of the forces. The Impulse is what counts so, as long as Fradial/ Ftan doesn't vary over the impact due to some time slipping and some without slipping during contact, the actual time profile of the forces is not too important. Its could be that the initial contact would not have any slipping as the angle will be steeper and the ball will be rotating a bit during the impact.
 
  • #160
A.T. said:
To confirm that linear and angular velocity changes are consistent with the assumption of a single impulse from the cue, check if the following relation holds:

w/v = 5/2 * r/R2

or:

w/v = 5/2 * sin(θ)/R

where:

w : angular speed
v : speed
r : lever arm of the force around the center
R : radius of the sphere
θ : angle between contact normal and the ball velocity / net force.

Thank you for posting this equation again. I calculated it and the result is pretty good. My understanding is that this means that we don't need to think other force than the impulse, which is awesome. But, we cannot say that the direction of the ball is near straight from this, right?

Just for your information, I will note the values used for the calculation. I measured these values in the video in which I hit a ball hung in the air (240fps).

w : 0.113 rad/ms (I used a red dot on the ball to measure this)
v : 4.55 mm/ms (linear speed of the ball just after collision)
R : 28.575 mm (known radius of the pocket ball)
θ : 15° (it is hard to know the actual angle, but it is in the range of 15-25deg)

The result is 0.024835 = 0.022644. If values w and v are happen to be correct, the actual hit angle would be ~16.4°.
 
Last edited:
  • #161
I was not familiar with the equation A.T. posted, but I took that the equation indicates that torque is just a byproduct of the force for linear acceleration. This is good to know. Then, maybe can I calculate the tangential force for linear acceleration from the angular speed (applied torque) and the length of the lever arm? maybe I am just confused...
 
  • #162
Wait... if the cue applies force F to the ball, is the normal force Fcosθ and tangential force Fsinθ? And if all the tangential force Fsinθ can be used for linear acceleration to the ball due to the friction, is it obvious that the ball goes straight?
 
  • #163
poolplayer said:
But, we cannot say that the direction of the ball is near straight from this, right?
What do you mean by "goes straight"? Parallel to the cue? Then no, the cue orientation and velocity don't come into this, just the cue contact position and the resulting ball velocities. It confirms that no other forces than the cue contact are relevant, but it doesn't go into how cue velocity / orientation lead to that force.
 
  • #164
A.T. said:
What do you mean by "goes straight"? Parallel to the cue? Then no, the cue orientation and velocity don't come into this, just the cue contact position and the resulting ball velocities. It confirms that no other forces than the cue contact are relevant, but it doesn't go into how cue velocity / orientation lead to that force.
Yes, that was what I meant.

I am quite confused now, but if my last post is one way to understand why the ball goes straight (parallel to the cue), it was really easy question to answer. And I don't know why my experiments suggested that cue flex is important for determining the ball direction.
 
  • #165
poolplayer said:
I don't know why my experiments suggested that cue flex is important for determining the ball direction.

Think of it as adding gradually less important factors (or terms...) to the model. The basic model is a cue and cueball made of frictionless adamantium, in a vacuum, without bridge hand. This is the 15 degree scenario from the first page, and the cue would bounce off to the side uninhibited. But the contact is not frictionless, so it's much less than 15 degrees, let's say 0.8 degrees. The bridge hand and back hand inhibit the cue bouncing off sideways, this transfers more lateral force to the ball, so we're at maybe 1.1 degrees. The cue flexes however, which is akin to letting the cue bounce off sideways just a little. We're back at 0.9 degrees because of that. The resistance of the ball against the cloth, amplified by the slightly downward cue stroke, means that the cueball resists the sideways force somewhat compared to the vacuum. That brings it back lower, say 0.7 degrees. The longitudinal friction of the cloth and the downward angle tilt the rotation axis forward, which curves the path slightly, also due to cloth friction. By the time the cueball hits the object ball, maybe it's only 0.5 degrees anymore.

Any more marginal factors we've discovered over 9 pages that I missed?
 
  • #166
David VH said:
Any more marginal factors we've discovered over 9 pages that I missed?
Thank you for summarizing it. I think that is quite much, although I have been at a loss at the most basic level 2) (please see the quote below)...

Is large friction between the ball and cue sufficient that the ball goes parallel to the cue? What still confuses me is some videos that I posted before. I hit the ball with a metal cue (so small cue flex. tip is normal, so there should be the same friction as a normal cue) and it seems that the ball went rather to the normal direction. Do you think this is just a sort of double hit? It seems that the ball already started to go oblique immediately after the first contact.


David VH said:
1) The basic model is a cue and cueball made of frictionless adamantium, in a vacuum, without bridge hand. This is the 15 degree scenario from the first page
2) But the contact is not frictionless, so it's much less than 15 degrees, let's say 0.8 degrees.
3) The bridge hand and back hand inhibit the cue bouncing off sideways, this transfers more lateral force to the ball, so we're at maybe 1.1 degrees.
4) The cue flexes however, which is akin to letting the cue bounce off sideways just a little. We're back at 0.9 degrees because of that.
5) The resistance of the ball against the cloth, amplified by the slightly downward cue stroke, means that the cueball resists the sideways force somewhat compared to the vacuum. That brings it back lower, say 0.7 degrees.
6) The longitudinal friction of the cloth and the downward angle tilt the rotation axis forward, which curves the path slightly, also due to cloth friction. By the time the cueball hits the object ball, maybe it's only 0.5 degrees anymore.
I don't know if these values are reasonable, but my intuition says changes of the ball direction due to these factors would be correct.
 
  • #167
poolplayer said:
And if all the tangential force Fsinθ can be used for linear acceleration to the ball due to the friction, is it obvious that the ball goes straight?
Friction explains why the ball doesn't move along the contact normal. But nothing implies that the ball must move parallel to the cue (and as far I can see it doesn't).
 
  • #168
A.T. said:
Friction explains why the ball doesn't move along the contact normal. But nothing implies that the ball must move parallel to the cue (and as far I can see it doesn't).
OK. Let me confirm this. When the cue applies net force (F) to the ball, is the tangential force Fsinθ? If so, do you think tangential acceleration of the ball is F/m sinθ if there is enough friction? (m: mass of the ball)
 
  • #169
poolplayer said:
When the cue applies net force (F) to the ball, is the tangential force Fsinθ?
If θ is the angle between F and contact normal, then Fsinθ is the tangential force per definition.

poolplayer said:
If so, do you think tangential acceleration of the ball is F/m sinθ if there is enough friction? (m: mass of the ball)
If Fsinθ is the tangential force then F/m sinθ is the tangential acceleration (assuming no other relevant forces than the cue contact).
 
  • #170
A.T. said:
If Fsinθ is the tangential force then F/m sinθ is the tangential acceleration (assuming no other relevant forces than the cue contact).
And then, in theory the ball goes parallel to the cue direction, doesn't it? Because the normal acceleration of the ball is F/m cosθ.
 
  • #171
A.T. said:
If θ is the angle between F and contact normal, then Fsinθ is the tangential force per definition.

If Fsinθ is the tangential force then F/m sinθ is the tangential acceleration (assuming no other relevant forces than the cue contact).

poolplayer said:
And then, in theory the ball goes parallel to the cue direction, doesn't it?

I have no idea how you come to this conclusion, since nothing I wrote above references the cue direction in any way.
 
  • #172
A.T. said:
I have no idea how you come to this conclusion, since nothing I wrote above references the cue direction in any way.
I think cue direction only changes θ.
 
  • #173
A.T. said:
I have no idea how you come to this conclusion, since nothing I wrote above references the cue direction in any way.
I think I got what you said. I meant that the directions of the cue and F are the same, but you meant that the direction of F is the direction of the ball acceleration and not necessarily the cue direction. I think you are right... anyway, it seems that you don't think that the ball always goes parallel to the cue direction even if there is enough friction between the cue and ball and there is no other force than collision.
 
  • #174
poolplayer said:
I meant that the directions of the cue and F are the same, but you meant that the direction of F is the direction of the ball acceleration and not necessarily the cue direction.
Yes, exactly. If F is the only relevant force, then the velocity will be parallel to it's average value during impact. This is the simple part.

poolplayer said:
I think you are right... anyway, it seems that you don't think that the ball always goes parallel to the cue direction even if there is enough friction between the cue and ball and there is no other force than collision.
At least I don't see an obvious reason why this should always be true. But maybe there is a combo of friction coefficients and other parameters where it goes parallel or even to the other side. This is the complex part.
 
  • #175
poolplayer said:
we don't need to think other force than the impulse, which is awesome.
That assumption is fine as long as the contact is brief. 'Stroking' along the side may well affect the accuracy of that approximation. Would I be right unsaying that stroking is sometimes used for some shots?
 
  • #176
sophiecentaur said:
'Stroking' along the side may well affect the accuracy of that approximation. Would I be right unsaying that stroking is sometimes used for some shots?
Some players including professional players use that kind of shots when they have to hit with right/left English. I think it is not like stroking or scratching on the ball, but rather an attempt not to push against the impact of cue-to-ball collision, which results in a sudden change of the cue direction to the side during/after the collision. They believe that it makes the ball go more straight (more parallel to the initial cue direction). Massive 'stroking' might be used in some masse shots, but this is not related to this thread.

This kind of shots may affect the ball direction. However, many people do not use that shot, but still can make a ball go almost straight with right/left English. So, I guess its effect would be small, maybe ~1-2° at most. I don't use that shot because it hurts consistency of shots.
 
  • #177
A.T. said:
Yes, exactly. If F is the only relevant force, then the velocity will be parallel to it's average value during impact. This is the simple part.
You are absolutely right.. but this is a kind of tautology. When I was asking about applied force F by the cue, I was talking about a simple case in theory where some unknown parameters can be ignored.

When force F (whose direction is off the normal) is applied to a ball, is the tangential acceleration of the ball F/m sinθ if there is enough friction between the force and ball (enough friction means Fsinθ < μFcosθ , where μ is static friction coefficient)?

Is this an ill-posed problem? If not and if it is right, maybe this was the simplest answer that I could get. My problem was that I thought that all of the tangential force Fsinθ cannot be used for linear acceleration (I even thought that the tangential force only spins a ball...). But, it seems that all the force can be used for linear acceleration of center-of-mass after a little research. (if there is enough friction on the contact surface)
1) Application: pull on two hockey pucks
https://books.google.co.jp/books?id=8oyNPd5QbYgC&pg=PA350&lpg=PA350&dq=puck+string+center+of+mass+velocity+google+book&source=bl&ots=61cEZe4Zaj&sig=blaDAk_ZGOhw0PbgPsm-6To0X7U&hl=en&sa=X&ved=0ahUKEwisivLSvLPRAhWr1IMKHVcaC68Q6AEIIzAB#v=onepage&q=puck string center of mass velocity google book&f=false
2) Puck-to-stick collision and fusion on frictionless ice
http://web.mit.edu/8.01t/www/materials/InClass/IC_Sol_W11D2-4.pdf

Of course, we now know that some factors such as cue flex or player's arm movement do not guarantee that the force is always in the direction of the cue during the collision, which makes the case complex. But, I would say the applied force is almost always near to the initial direction of the cue. Because players usually try to push a cue straight forward.
 
Last edited:
  • #178
poolplayer said:
When force F (whose direction is off the normal) is applied to a ball, is the tangential acceleration of the ball F/m sinθ ...
Yes.

poolplayer said:
But, it seems that all the force can be used for linear acceleration of center-of-mass after a little research.
Yes.
 
  • Like
Likes poolplayer
  • #179
I think I am done here if no one points to a flaw in the logic. Thank you.
poolplayer said:
What still confuses me is some videos that I posted before. I hit the ball with a metal cue (so small cue flex. tip is normal, so there should be the same friction as a normal cue) and it seems that the ball went rather to the normal direction. Do you think this is just a sort of double hit? It seems that the ball already started to go oblique immediately after the first contact.

This video is still mysterious, though... maybe some unidentified leftwards force makes the ball go significantly off from parallel to the cue direction, but I will think about this in other occasions.
 
  • #180
poolplayer said:
Because players usually try to push a cue straight forward.
I believe that is true but that doesn't imply that there is no lateral force from the cue (the fact that it requires some effort must imply there is some lateral force). So how could one measure the actual impulse? Answer has to be by observing the actual direction of the resulting motion of the ball when hung on a string. You have already done that on a few (or one) occasions and you would really need to do several runs (>10) with the same contact point etc. and analyse the result by taking the mean after discarding any that don't fit the pattern. It may need a mechanical actuator, rather than a human. Also, it would be interesting to find out the coeff of friction, which could be dominated by the cue tip, so a flat ceramic plate could be used with a cue tip under a known mass and a spring balance used to pull it along the surface of the plate. There have been various values suggested but I wonder how well justified they are. Varying the applied weight could show how linear the friction is - which could really spoil your day if you wanted to make real sense of it.
 
  • #181
poolplayer said:
maybe some unidentified leftwards force
. . . or lack of friction force between tip and ball. No friction would mean only a normal force would be applied.
 
  • #182
sophiecentaur said:
I believe that is true but that doesn't imply that there is no lateral force from the cue (the fact that it requires some effort must imply there is some lateral force). So how could one measure the actual impulse? Answer has to be by observing the actual direction of the resulting motion of the ball when hung on a string. You have already done that on a few (or one) occasions and you would really need to do several runs (>10) with the same contact point etc. and analyse the result by taking the mean after discarding any that don't fit the pattern. It may need a mechanical actuator, rather than a human. Also, it would be interesting to find out the coeff of friction, which could be dominated by the cue tip, so a flat ceramic plate could be used with a cue tip under a known mass and a spring balance used to pull it along the surface of the plate. There have been various values suggested but I wonder how well justified they are. Varying the applied weight could show how linear the friction is - which could really spoil your day if you wanted to make real sense of it.
I agree that these are necessary experiments to know the actual force applied by the cue. But, I am now satisfied by the simple model because in the model the ball always goes straight.
 
  • #183
sophiecentaur said:
. . . or lack of friction force between tip and ball. No friction would mean only a normal force would be applied.
Yeah, this might be possible. Even if the cue tip is the same as a usual cue (so the friction coefficient should be the same in the simplest case), there may be nonlinearity of friction coefficient as a function of amount of force. If the force applied by the metal cue is too strong due to the larger effective mass of the cue, the tangential force might have exceeded the static friction. I cannot know the amount of slippage even though slippage is not apparent in the video. This is a good point.
 
  • #184
poolplayer said:
even though slippage is not apparent in the video
Contact is only brief. Would your frame rate show up the actual contact time? I reckon you'd need to find a way of actually measuring the friction. Perhaps with ball secured in a hole and the cue resting on top (with added weights) measure force needed to shift the tip.
Question: When players chalk their cue so regularly, is it for a physical reason or just a ritual to help them settle to each shot? Also, when the commentator says they got a 'kick', what is meant? Is it the chalk sticking to the ball?
 
  • #185
sophiecentaur said:
Contact is only brief. Would your frame rate show up the actual contact time?
You are right. I cannot know the contact time and slippage from my 240fps video.

sophiecentaur said:
When players chalk their cue so regularly, is it for a physical reason or just a ritual to help them settle to each shot?
If I shoot a ball several times (<10-20) without putting additional chalk, the cue readily slips on the ball (miscue) if my shot is off center. This is because a significant amount of chalk is removed from the cue tip in every shot.

sophiecentaur said:
Also, when the commentator says they got a 'kick', what is meant? Is it the chalk sticking to the ball?
It means that a ball went to unusual direction and many think that it is due to accidentally increased friction between balls. I think it is mostly due to the chalk on the ball. But, I heard that a clean new ball often induces a 'kick' in an unpredictable way because the friction is sometimes higher on the surface of a clean ball.
 
  • #186
sophiecentaur said:
Question: When players chalk their cue so regularly, is it for a physical reason or just a ritual to help them settle to each shot?

It is both. If you forget to chalk before a significantly off-center shot, you risk a miscue. It's easy to forget how long it's been since you chalked it, so many players have developed a habit of chalking while they plan the shot so they never forget, turning it into a ritual. 90% of billiard chalk is being wasted though, that's for sure (and it's also a health hazard).
 
  • #187
David VH said:
It is both. If you forget to chalk before a significantly off-center shot, you risk a miscue. It's easy to forget how long it's been since you chalked it, so many players have developed a habit of chalking while they plan the shot so they never forget, turning it into a ritual. 90% of billiard chalk is being wasted though, that's for sure (and it's also a health hazard).
So the chalk increases the effect of friction between the surfaces and probably makes it more consistent. It wouldn't be hard to do the measurement I was suggesting, though. I do not have a cue or a snooker ball so I can't help there. It is easy for me to suggest a whole range of possible experiments but what about a set of measurements with different amounts of offset (English). You're going to need a bigger memory stick!
 
  • #188
This video may be of interest to this thread:


The most interesting part IMO is around the 5:55 mark where consecutive shots are being performed, depleting the chalk from the cue. You can see that the cueball deflection from the cue direction exceeds 1 degree, time after time.

From this video I learned that the deflection of the cueball from the cue direction is called "squirt". This led me to this very comprehensive page:
http://billiards.colostate.edu/threads/squirt.html

This page basically confirms our findings, and comes up with the same influencing factors (e.g. immediate swerve because of downward cueing angle). To summarize: our minds work pretty well, but our google-fu is below par.
 
  • #189
Any computer simulation programs for this game ?
 
  • #190
This subject is more complicated than most of these answers have suggested. It is pretty well understood by serious practitioners of the billiards arts. For a good introduction see the links at this website: http://billiards.colostate.edu/threads/squirt.html. Basically there are a number of effects that contribute to the cueball motion after being struck by a cuestick. These are

1. Deflection or squirt
2. Swerve ( massé is an extreme form: )
3. Throw
4. Bounce

The flexibility and mass profile of the cue stick make a difference, as does the tip hardness and the type of stroke and english is used.

An early but rather complete treatise on billiards physics was done by Coriolis: http://www.coriolisbilliards.com/. A number of other texts have appeared since then delving into the classical mechanics underlying billiards.

Another good source of information are the articles by Bob Jewett over the years in Billiards Digest: http://www.sfbilliards.com/articles/BD_articles.html

Bob is arguably one of the leading experts in the world on the physics of billiards. The easiest way to understand these effects is to play the game.
 
  • #191
It is so weird that these billiard lovers do not give a straight answer to my original question. These links are of some help, but I could not find an answer probably because my question is too basic.

But, I think I got a simple answer in this thread:
It is *friction* between the cue tip and cue ball that makes the cue ball go almost straight (parallel to the cue direction) rather than to the oblique/normal direction (much deflection/squirt) predicted by the Newton's law. With enough friction, the tangential force can accelerate the cue ball as well as the normal force. The vector sum of the tangential force and normal force is in theory parallel to the cue direction, so the ball goes straight.
 
Last edited:
  • #192
poolplayer said:
It is so weird that these billiard lovers do not give a straight answer to my original question. These links are of some help, but I could not find an answer probably because my question is too basic.

But, I think I got a simple answer in this thread:
It is *friction* between the cue tip and cue ball that makes the cue ball go almost straight (parallel to the cue direction) rather than to the oblique/normal direction (much deflection/squirt) predicted by the Newton's law. With enough friction, the tangential force can accelerate the cue ball as well as the normal force. The vector sum of the tangential force and normal force is in theory parallel to the cue direction, so the ball goes straight.
As a billiard player for most of my life I will say that the ball does not go straight when struck with right or left English.
It squirts off line in the opposite direction of the english. The heavier the tip of the cue stick the more exaggerated this squirt is. Many companies have been working towards creating low deflection shafts for cues which minimize this squirt, and the key component in these is reducing the weight at the tip of the cue.

I hit with a cue that had a lead weighted tip and when putting any type of side spin on the cue it would shoot off at a sever angle from the aim point.
 
  • #193
DWT said:
As a billiard player for most of my life I will say that the ball does not go straight when struck with right or left English.
We all know that. Please read the thread first...
My question was why the ball goes more straight in cue-to-ball collision than the ball-to-ball collision.

english-jpg.110620.jpg


And my answer is the friction between the cue tip and cue ball makes the cue ball go almost straight (parallel to the cue direction) rather than to the oblique/normal direction (much deflection/squirt) predicted by the Newton's law. With enough friction, the tangential force can accelerate the cue ball as well as the normal force. The vector sum of the tangential force and normal force is in theory parallel to the cue direction, so the ball goes straight.
 
  • Like
Likes sophiecentaur
  • #194
Put a tip on the cue made of the same material as the balls and as long as the weight of the tip is less than the object ball, i guarantee you the ball will still follow the almost straight direction.

It is not due to friction. As a matter of fact the harder the tip the straighter the ball goes as long as you don't miss cue.

One thing that happens at impact is the cue shaft will flex away from the cue ball in the same direction of the english. This is where the loss in angular momentum is.
 
  • #195
There is a good exercise nearby by the way. Let ball moves on a horizontal table and there is a Coulomb friction between the ball and the surface. Show that the center of the ball describes a parabola until the slipping stops. What about slope?
 
Last edited:
  • #196
zwierz said:
What about slope?
The table should be flat unless you are referring to the nap of the cloth.
 
  • #197
I meant to consider the problem for inclined table. There is a ball on the inclined table; there is a Coulomb friction between the ball and the surface. Describe the motion of the ball while the ball slips. There is something to think about :)
 
  • #198
DWT said:
Put a tip on the cue made of the same material as the balls and as long as the weight of the tip is less than the object ball, i guarantee you the ball will still follow the almost straight direction.
Do you have links to videos demonstrating this with low friction tips?
 
  • #199
A.T. said:
Do you have links to videos demonstrating this with low friction tips?
I will work on one. Going to have to make tip and apply it to an old shaft may take a few days.

Until then you can search for using a phenolic tip. These are the lowest friction tips on the market. They were designed for breaking and jumping but a good practitioner can impart english to the cue ball without chalk.
The chances of a mis-cue are higher but with a good stroke it is doable.
 
  • #200
DWT said:
Put a tip on the cue made of the same material as the balls and as long as the weight of the tip is less than the object ball, i guarantee you the ball will still follow the almost straight direction.

It is not due to friction. As a matter of fact the harder the tip the straighter the ball goes as long as you don't miss cue.

One thing that happens at impact is the cue shaft will flex away from the cue ball in the same direction of the english. This is where the loss in angular momentum is.

Thank you for your answer. My thought is that as long as we don't miscue, there is enough friction between the tip and ball due to chalk.

But, I have never tried English with a phenolic tip without chalk. If we can make the ball go straight without miscue, it might not be friction because now the friction should be same as ball-to-ball collision. To avoid miscue I guess we can only strike around the center without chalk though.
 
Last edited:
Back
Top