Raghav Gupta
- 1,010
- 76
How the Mean colour of visible spectrum is yellow?
Shouldn't that be green according to acronym VIBGYOR?
Shouldn't that be green according to acronym VIBGYOR?
Raghav Gupta said:How the Mean colour of visible spectrum is yellow?
Shouldn't that be green according to acronym VIBGYOR?
Raghav Gupta said:How the Mean colour of visible spectrum is yellow?
Sure butmathman said:Above shows solar spectrum. Peak is in the green. It is asymmetric, higher at red end than at purple end.
There was a question in my practical manual-Drakkith said:I've never heard this before. Can you post a reference? It may help to get some context.
Raghav Gupta said:How the Mean colour of visible spectrum is yellow?
Shouldn't that be green according to acronym VIBGYOR?
The statement was vague. The boundaries of the visible spectrum are not exact. The statement was not about mean wavelength or mean frequency but about mean "color". All averages are weighted. It's just that the weights are often assumed to be uniform. But uniform by what measure? A uniform weight by frequency will give a different mean than a uniform weight by wavelength.DaveC426913 said:Sure but
a] question was not about solar spectrum, just visible spectrum, and
b] question was not about average (i.e. weighted), it is about mean (i.e. highest minus lowest).
Either way it's a fabrication.2lmilehi said:I've understood the acronym as ROY G BIV
Raghav Gupta said:I had a doubt as green colour lies in between the visible spectrum. The mean wavelength and mean frequency of visible spectrum should be green?
sophiecentaur said:Why consider the mean wavelength when you could just as easily consider the mean frequency? Would you get the same answer? :) (Harmonic mean)
DaveC426913 said:b] question was not about average (i.e. weighted), it is about mean (i.e. highest minus lowest).
Arithmetic mean of what? It is only arbitrary and historical that we measure the wavelength of light in preference to the frequency. In fact, the Chemistry of what goes on in our eye receptors will be frequency based and not wavelength based. (i.e. photon energies)Raghav Gupta said:I think we usually take arithmetic mean in these cases.
Then both have same mean wavelength and mean frequency.
sophiecentaur said:Arithmetic mean of what? It is only arbitrary and historical that we measure the wavelength of light in preference to the frequency. In fact, the Chemistry of what goes on in our eye receptors will be frequency based and not wavelength based. (i.e. photon energies)
You should try with some different random values before you make a statement like that.
In general, the harmonic mean is not the same as the mean of a set of numbers.
(A +B)/2 is not the same as 1/((1/A + 1/B)/2), which is what you are claiming.
The frequency is inversely proportional to the wavelength so, if you want the same answer for both, you need to take the harmonic mean for one and the arithmetic mean for the other. I was originally making the point that using a mean wavelength is an arbitrary choice.Raghav Gupta said:I know arithmetic mean is different from harmonic mean.
I am not claiming that both are equal.
I was kind of asking that why we have to take harmonic mean instead of arithmetic mean when we have to find mean frequency of visible spectrum?
I thought till Drakkith's reply I was understanding most of the things.
He gave also the reason that yellow might be the mean colour considered because of sodium history.
So if all in reality is frequency based according to your quote
Then why harmonic mean?
sophiecentaur said:The frequency is inversely proportional to the wavelength so, if you want the same answer for both, you need to take the harmonic mean for one and the arithmetic mean for the other. I was originally making the point that using a mean wavelength is an arbitrary choice.
Also, there really is no such thing as a Mean Colour (a very fuzzy quantity, at best). There is no future in a conversation that tries to relate what we perceive to the spectrum of incident light unless you are prepared to include how the three sensors will respond, separately, to the black body spectrum and then plot the resultant (processed) signal values onto a CIE chart. But you don't need to do that sum, to know the answer and that is - You Won't See Green. Every day you do that experiment when you look at sunlight reflected on white surfaces.
Indigo corresponds to that. According to thisjbriggs444 said:What frequency corresponds to a wavelength of 495.238 nm?
How does this frequency compare to the arithmetic mean of the upper and lower frequencies of the visible range?
What I and others were fishing for was to get you thinking about the relationship between arithmetic mean, harmonic mean, weavelength and frequency.Raghav Gupta said:Indigo corresponds to that.
Violet wavelength as 400nm and red wavelength as 650 nm.
When we use arithmetic mean here we get 525 nm as A.M wavelength which is of green.
Now taking the same numbers, calculated the H.M
Got it as 495.238 nm
So why my practical manual is messing here.Raghav Gupta said:There was a question in my practical manual-
" In general for which colour we take the refractive index of a material in lens and glass slabs.""
The answer was given
Yellow colour. Since it is the mean colour of visible spectrum.
Okay got it. So we really get same answers either way. I have not thought of dividing that wavelengths by c . Thanksjbriggs444 said:What I and others were fishing for was to get you thinking about the relationship between arithmetic mean, harmonic mean, weavelength and frequency.
What is the frequency corresponding to 400 nm? Divide the speed of light by 400 nm and you get around 750 terahertz.
What is the frequency corresponding to 650 nm? Divide the speed of light by 650 nm and you get around 461 terahertz.
What is the arithmetic mean of those two frequencies? Add and divide by two and you get around 605.5 terahertz.
What wavelength does that frequency correspond to? Divide the speed of light by 605.5 terahertz and you get around 495 nm.
By no coincidence, 495 nm is the harmonic mean of 400 nm and 650 nm.
There is no point in complaining about or getting confused by one book ("practical manual"?). You have to read round other sources and have discussions like this one - but you need to go to a reputable forum like PF. It's a common problem that people who try to write books to simplify things will get facts wrong by over-simplifying. The way to describe a colour that lies somewhere near the middle of the optical spectrum is to call it an 'average' colour, which is a catch-all and non-specific term for 'middle'. If you say "mean" then you imply a particular mathematical operation that cannot be done with written Colour Names.Raghav Gupta said:So why my practical manual is messing here.
Does it is not upto the standards.
Well I agree it is kind of classifying
And not teaching us to understand.
Please I know that the term mean colour is again coming here.
Just want a comment for this quote.
The most important aspect of the eye's response is that it has virtually no wavelength resolving facility - never mind its amplitude response. It is not a spectrometer.mp3car said:our eyes do not have a linear response to equal power of different wavelengths
Shane Kennedy said:It also depends on how you measure it. Can we actually measure the amplitude of the wave, or do we measure by it's power ?
Is there a device that can measure light without any colour bias ?
Drakkith said:Not of visible light. The wave alternates too fast for electronics to respond so we can't directly measure the amplitude of each alternation. Instead we measure the power over some amount of time.
Of course. A properly calibrated spectrometer is one such device.
When at college, learning about matrices, and since, I often wondered how you can get the value of a matrix. What defines what cell does what, and where does the information come from. A practical example would be good.sophiecentaur said:I wonder what would be the arithmetic mean of Tom, Dick and Harry?
It is quite possible to use 'superheterodyne' (now there's a lovely old word) methods to beat down a CW signal from a laser to a manageable RF frequency and to 'look at' the variations of the fields in the light wave. You may say it's a bit of a cheat but no one (?) uses TRF (tuned radio frequency) receivers these days, so we do the same trick for RF measurement, in any case.Drakkith said:Not of visible light. The wave alternates too fast for electronics to respond so we can't directly measure the amplitude of each alternation. Instead we measure the power over some amount of time.
Of course. A properly calibrated spectrometer is one such device.
All human senses are like this. With the exception of highly trained craftsmen and people with perfect pitch, all quantities have to be measured with instruments, if high accuracy is needed. I think you may still be missing the point that the eye is not a spectrometer. It can make a stab at estimating the wavelength of a single spectral line but it is completely unable to resolve the spectral components of any other light source. All it can do is to 'name the colour' of an object or light source - which is nothing like the spectral content. It is because of this that Colour TV works.Shane Kennedy said:Is there a device that can measure light without any colour bias ? I am sure that human eyes vary slightly in their sensitivity too.
Shane Kennedy said:I am sure that human eyes vary slightly in their sensitivity too.
You are using terms in colour synthesis here but colour analysis is not suited to those descriptions. It s a bit simplistic to describe the receptors as 'red', 'green' and 'blue' receptors because they are all sensitive to more or less the whole visual spectrum. This is essential for the way they work.fizixfan said:Good point Shane. I'm what you call "color blind," although "red-green deficient" or better yet "deuteranopia" are more accurate terms.
We have red, green and blue color receptors (cones) in our retinas. I probably have fewer red and green receptors than people with normal vision do.
But - and this is just a theory - color blind and color deficient people may have more rods (for light sensitivity, not color) than non-color blind people, and so may have better night vision. My night vision is pretty good.
I also wonder if the RGB cones distinguish colors and hues via a RGB (additive) or CMYK (subtractive) process. Probably additive, since computer screens use this method, whereas the color printing (pigmenting) process uses the subtractive colors.
sophiecentaur said:The most important aspect of the eye's response is that it has virtually no wavelength resolving facility - never mind its amplitude response. It is not a spectrometer.
There is no way that the eye can distinguish between a Yellow Sodium line and an appropriate mix of Green and Red monochromatic light. I thing that demonstrates pretty well that the eye cannot resolve different wavelengths. I can't think why people feel it necessary to defend the abilities of the eye by suggesting it can do things that it can't Why should it matter?mp3car said:No wavelength resolving facility? No, it's not a spectrometer, but the various cone cells respond differently when we view one wavelength compared to another. No, we can't tell a difference between 532nm and 533nm, nor a difference between a surface illuminated with 70W/m^2 vs. 71W/m^2...
However, if a surface is emitting monochromatic 400nm, I am going to have a different cone response than I would it were 600nm. And as has been said, the perceived brightness depends not only on power, but also the wavelength. I am not sure what you mean by "no wavelength resolving facility" considering we can easily resolve a difference between the two different wavelengths in this example. As I said, of course we can't tell 532nm vs. 533 and maybe even 532 and 540nm. But in the big picture, taking the entire system into account of the eye and brain, 500nm and 600nm are "resolved" very differently (I am using the term "resolve" to mean separate, distinguish, discern).
I'm not sure what you are implying about the way the sensors work but the accepted theory is that each of the three sensors have a very wide band sensitivity. Taken on its own, a sensor will not 'know' whether it is receiving low level light of wavelength near its peak of sensitivity or high level light way off its peak. It is only when the brain has information about all three sensors that a sense of 'Colour' (not necessarily wavelength) can be deduced by comparing the relative output levels of the sensors. Dispersion doesn't come into it. Indeed, how can it with non spectral incident light?blue_leaf77 said:All materials must exhibit dispersion (different response to frequency), this is guaranteed by Kramers-Kronig relation. The only way a "medium" behavior can be independent of frequency is that to have unit refractive index (again proved by Kramers-Kronig relation) which means vacuum, so the eye being sensitive to different wavelength is a must. Probably the more appropriate way to get around this discrepancy is to introduce resolution for the eye. Anyway what we call colors is actually how our brain gives its response to incoming stimulus, it doesn't output numbers like spectrometers. Therefore we don't have to associate eye to spectrometers in all ways, only in some ways that it's sensitive to different wavelength.
sophiecentaur said:You are using terms in colour synthesis here but colour analysis is not suited to those descriptions. It s a bit simplistic to describe the receptors as 'red', 'green' and 'blue' receptors because they are all sensitive to more or less the whole visual spectrum. This is essential for the way they work.
This link (and dozens others from Google) tells you the main points about the tristimulus colour vision theory. That theory works well enough for Colour TV and other displays to work very well. Those displays work on additive mixing and give pretty good colour fidelity. within their gamut. Subtractive mixing (colour film and colour printing) is not so good if you can only use three primaries on their own. Spot colours can be used in printing to improve reproducibility (e.g. the Red in the CocaCola adverts would never be done 'right' with a dot matrix printer)
sophiecentaur said:On the subject of variations on the basic tristumulus system, it would be interesting to know whether it is racially differentiated, too.